Insightcast AI
Home
© 2025 All rights reserved
Impressum
#274 – Karl Deisseroth: Depression, Schizophrenia, and Psychiatry

#274 – Karl Deisseroth: Depression, Schizophrenia, and Psychiatry

Lex Fridman Podcast XX

--:--
--:--

Full Transcription:

[0] The following is a conversation with Carl Diceroth, Professor of Bioengineering, Psychiatry, and Behavioral Sciences at Stanford University.

[1] He's one of the greatest living psychiatrist and neuroscientists in the world.

[2] He's also just a fascinating human being.

[3] We discuss both the darkest and the most beautiful places that the human mind can take us.

[4] He explores this in his book called Projections, a Story of Human Emotions.

[5] I highly recommend it.

[6] It's written masterfully.

[7] And now a quick few second mention of each sponsor.

[8] Check them out in the description.

[9] It's the best way to support this podcast.

[10] We got Buy Optimizers for health, better help for your mind, notion for productivity, blink is for insights into your favorite non -fiction books and Magic Spoon for delicious, delicious, healthy breakfast.

[11] Choose wise and my friends.

[12] And now onto the full ad reads.

[13] As always, no ads in the middle.

[14] I try to make this interesting, but if you skip them, if you must, please still check out our sponsors.

[15] I enjoy their stuff.

[16] Maybe you will too.

[17] This show is brought to you by buy optimizers, B -I -optimizers that have a new magnesium supplement.

[18] When I fast, I'm doing keto or carnivore, sodium, potassium, and magnesium are essential.

[19] Magnesium, the thing I take when I fast, is tricky to get right.

[20] That's why I use magnesium breakthrough from bi -optimizers.

[21] Most supplements contain only one or two forms of magnesium, like glycinate or citrate, when in reality there are at least seven that your body needs and benefits from.

[22] You can learn all about this, actually, from Andrew Huberman, and you should definitely go listen to this podcast called Huberman Lab podcast.

[23] He's an incredible human, full of deep knowledge and wisdom and information.

[24] that's actionable for your life.

[25] Go to magbreakthrough .com slash Lex for a special discount.

[26] That's magbreakthrough .com slash Lex.

[27] M -A -G -breakthrough .com slash Lex.

[28] This episode is also brought to you by BetterHelp.

[29] Spelled H -E -L -P -Help, just like you would write it out.

[30] If you were on a deserted island looking at a volleyball with a handprint named Wilson, they figure out Better Help that is.

[31] figure out what you need and match you with a licensed professional therapist in under 48 hours.

[32] Every time I say 48 hours, it gives me flashbacks.

[33] And it's doing the 4x4 by 48 challenge from David Goggins, where you run 4 miles every 4 hours.

[34] David is one of the people that I go to for my inner turmoil that eventually materializes into inner peace.

[35] He is a kind of therapist for me. if I may say so, but without so much talking.

[36] Now, I also believe in talking as a method for therapy, and for that, you should use BetterHelp.

[37] It's easy, private, affordable, available worldwide.

[38] Check them out at betterhelp .com slash Lex.

[39] That's betterhelp .com slash Lex.

[40] This show is also brought to you by Notion, a note -taking and team collaboration tool.

[41] It combines note -taking, document sharing, wikis, project management, and much more.

[42] all in one space, that simple, powerful, and beautifully designed.

[43] Everybody I talked to, okay, that might be a little bit of an exaggeration.

[44] I think I tweeted all generalizations are wrong, including this one.

[45] But they're also fun, and they're also useful.

[46] Anyway, the generalization I was about to say is that people that I talk to all love notion.

[47] That's like the cutting edge, that's the state of the art, the sexiest note -taking app.

[48] Now, the thing they want me to tell you is that, it's not just for individuals, it's also for startups.

[49] It can provide a full -on what they're calling operating system for running every aspect of your company as it grows quickly.

[50] They're running a special offer just for startups and they will give you a thousand bucks off of Notion's team plan.

[51] If you go to notion .com slash startups.

[52] They give you a sense.

[53] That's almost a year of free Notion for a team of 10.

[54] Go to notion .com slash startups.

[55] That's notion .com slash startups.

[56] This show is brought to you by Blinkist, my favorite app for learning new things.

[57] I'm sorry I'm running around the mic.

[58] I just get really excited when I'm doing these ad reads.

[59] Blinkist takes the key ideas from thousands of nonfiction books and condenses them down into just 15 minutes that you can read or listen to.

[60] I can recommend a million books from sapiens to meditations by Marcus Aurelius.

[61] but what I should more specifically or more generally say that's a contradiction is that you can use it to both review the books you've already read or to select books that you're going to read and both are super useful.

[62] Go to blinkus .com slash Lex to start your free seven -day trial and get 25 % off of a Blinkist premium membership.

[63] That's Blinkist .com slash Lex spelled B -L -I -N -K -I -S -T.

[64] Blinkets .com slash Lex.

[65] This episode is also brought to you by Magic Spoon.

[66] Just saying that brings happiness to my heart.

[67] It's a low carb, keto -friendly cereal.

[68] It has zero grams of sugar, 13 to 14 grams of protein, only 4 9 grams of carbs, 140 calories in each serving.

[69] I just had a huge amount of barbecue, Texas barbecue yesterday.

[70] I made myself feel a lot better today.

[71] to balance it out with the lightness and the health of also a keto -friendly meal, which is Magic Spoon.

[72] You can build your own box or get a variety pack, but ladies and gentlemen, the best flavor is cocoa.

[73] It is the flavor of champions.

[74] It reminds me of childhood and the happiness I felt when I was eating the sweet, sweet, delicious cereal.

[75] But at the time, those cereals were full of sugar, and this cereal is not.

[76] So it's also healthy for you.

[77] Magic Spoon have a 100 % happiness guarantee, so if you don't like it, they refund it.

[78] Go to magic spoon .com slash Lex and use code Lex at checkout to save five bucks off your order.

[79] That's magic spoon .com slash Lex and use code Lex.

[80] This is the Lex Friedman podcast, and here is my conversation with Carl Diceroth.

[81] You open your book called Projections, a story of human emotions with a few beautiful words to summarize all of humanity.

[82] The book draws insights about the human mind for modern psychiatry and neuroscience.

[83] So if it's okay, let me read a few sentences from the opening.

[84] Go ahead.

[85] You got to give props to beautiful writing when I see it.

[86] Quote, In the art of weaving, warp threads are structural and strong and anchored at the origin, creating a frame for crossing fibers as the fabric is woven.

[87] Projecting across the advancing edge into free space, warp threads bridge the formed past, to the ragged present, to the yet featureless future.

[88] Yet featureless future.

[89] Well done.

[90] Well done, sir.

[91] The tapestry of the human story has its own warp threads, rooted deep in the gorges of East Africa, connecting the shifting textures of human life over millions of years.

[92] Spanning pictographs backdrop by creviced ice, by angulated forestry, by stone and steel, and by glowing rare earths.

[93] The inner workings of the mind give form to these threads, creating a framework within us, upon which the story of each individual can come into being.

[94] Personal grain and color arise from the cross threads of our moments and experiences, the fine weft of life, embedding and obscuring the underlying scaffold with intricate and sometimes lovely detail.

[95] Here are stories of this fabric fraying in those who are ill. in the minds of people for whom the warp is exposed and raw and revealing.

[96] What have you learned about human beings, human nature, and the human mind from those who suffer from psychiatric maladies for those for whom this fabric is warped?

[97] One thing we learn as biologists is that when something breaks, you see what the original unbroken part was for.

[98] And we see this in genetics, we see this in biochemistry.

[99] It's known that when you have a mutated gene, sometimes the gene is turned up in strength or turned down in strength, and that lets you see what it was originally for.

[100] You can infer true function from dysfunction.

[101] And this is a theme that I thought needed to be shared and needed to be made communicable to the lay public, to everybody, people who, which is, I think, almost all of us who think and care about the inner workings of our mind, but who also...

[102] care for those who have been suffering, who have mental health disorders, who face challenges.

[103] But then more broadly, it's a very much larger story than the present.

[104] There's a story to be told where the protagonist really is the human mind.

[105] And that was one thing I wanted to share as well in projections, is that broader story, but still anchored in the moment of patients, of people, of experiences of the moment.

[106] Is there a clear line between dysfunction and function, disorder, and order?

[107] This is always debated in psychiatry, probably more so than any other medical specialty.

[108] I'm a psychiatrist.

[109] I treat patients still.

[110] I see acutely ill people who come to the emergency room where there's no doubt that this is not something that's working well, where the manifestation of disease is so powerful, where the person is suffering so greatly, where they cannot.

[111] continue as they are.

[112] But of course it's a spectrum and there are people who are closer to the realm of being able to work okay in their jobs but suffer from some small dysfunction and everywhere in between.

[113] In psychiatry, we're careful to say we don't call it a disease or a disorder unless there's a disruption in social or occupational functioning.

[114] But of course, psychiatry has a long way to go in terms of developing quantitative tests.

[115] We don't have blood draws.

[116] We don't have imaging studies that we can use to diagnose.

[117] And so that line ultimately that you're asking about between order and disorder, function and dysfunction, it's operational of the moment.

[118] How are things working?

[119] Can we just like linger on the terms for a second?

[120] So disease, dysfunction, how careful should we be using those words?

[121] Can we just, even in this conversation from a sort of technical perspective but also a human perspective.

[122] How quick should we be in saying that schizophrenia, depression, autism, as we kind of go down across the spectrum of different maladies, just to use the word dysfunction and disease?

[123] I would say, to give ourselves license to capture the whole spectrum, let's say disorder, because that captures truly, I think, the essence of it, which is we need to talk about it when it's not working when there's disorder.

[124] And that's the fairest and, you know, most inclusive term to use.

[125] So is it fair to assume that basically every member of the human species suffers from a large number of disorders then?

[126] Well, we just have to pick which ones are debilitating for each person.

[127] You know, if you look at the numbers, there are, you know, if you look at how our mental health disorders are currently defined, you can look at population prevalence values for all these disorders, and you can come up with estimates that somebody will have a lifetime prevalence of having a psychiatric disorder that approaches 25 % or so.

[128] And so that's, and in some studies it could be more, some studies it could be less.

[129] Now, what do we do with that number?

[130] What does that mean?

[131] And in some ways, that's the essence of what I was hoping to approach with the book, is to reflect on this spectrum that exists for all the disorders.

[132] There is taking nothing away from the severity and the suffering that comes at the extreme end of these illnesses, but nearly every one of them exists on a spectrum of severity from nearly functional to completely dysfunctional, life -threatening, and even fatal.

[133] And so that number 25%, more or less, it doesn't capture that spectrum of severity.

[134] To linger that number, where do those numbers come from?

[135] Is it self -reported?

[136] Is it people who show up and say, I need help?

[137] Is it somebody else that points out that person needs help?

[138] Or is it like estimates to even go beyond that for people who don't ask for help that are suffering quietly alone?

[139] When you look at self -report numbers, then those numbers get even higher beyond 25 % or more.

[140] Those, the most rigorous studies are done with structured psychiatric interviews where people who are trained in eliciting symptoms carefully do complete psychiatric inventories of individuals.

[141] And these are time -consuming laborious studies that are not often repeated.

[142] When they're done, they're done well.

[143] But very often you'll see a report of something in the news of a very high number for some disorder or symptom.

[144] And very often, if it's shockingly high, that's coming from a self -report of a person.

[145] And so that's another issue that we have, again, take nothing away from the severity and reality and biological nature of these disorders, which are very genetic, very, you know, We understand that these are very biological, and yet we lack right now the lab tests and the blood draws to make the diagnoses.

[146] We'll talk about it, just how biological they are, because that too is a mystery.

[147] You know, in terms of from a perspective of how to probe into the disease, how to understand it, how to help it.

[148] So some of it could be in your biological, some of it could be just the dance of human emotion and interaction.

[149] and the it's like uh it will is love when it works and is love when it breaks down a biological or is it something else so we're going to talk about it but let me just like to linger in terms of disorder what about genius you know that sort of cliche saying like the madness and genius that they kind of dance together uh what about if the thing we see is disorder is actually genius on heard or misunderstood.

[150] Well, here again, the numbers help us, and here's where being rigorous and quantitative actually really helps.

[151] If you look at disorders like autism and bipolar disorder and eating disorders, anorexia and nervosa, for example, these, you know, particularly bipolar and anorexia, these can be fatal, they can cause immense suffering, but they are heavily genetic, all three of these, and what's very interesting is each one of these three is actually correlated positively, positively, with measures of intelligence, of educational attainment, and even of income.

[152] And so you look at this, severe disorders, in many cases, causing quite immense morbidity and mortality, and yet they are positively correlated at the population level with positive things.

[153] Can you say the three again, autism?

[154] Autism, anorexia, and bipolar disorder.

[155] Bipolar, right?

[156] What's that book?

[157] I forgot the book name.

[158] But is intelligence a burden?

[159] Well, you know, people can get into trouble when they think they're smarter than they are.

[160] I will say that.

[161] I don't know.

[162] Sometimes, like, in the deepest meaning of that statement, I think ignorance is bliss.

[163] I'm a big fan of Prince Michigan from The Idiot and Adhosh from Brother Karamaz.

[164] of optimism can be seen as naivety and dumbness, but I think it's a kind of deep intelligence, maybe inability to reason sort of about the mechanics of the world, but instead kind of feel the world.

[165] And it seems like that's one of the paths to happiness.

[166] There is how much you think versus how much you feel.

[167] This comes up all the time.

[168] In medicine, we encounter this all the time.

[169] When you're day after day, you encounter this.

[170] you know, the abyss of suffering from patients.

[171] How much do you let yourself feel or how much do you make it abstract and objective and try to make it clinical?

[172] And that range, how you're able to move yourself on that spectrum is very important for survival as a physician.

[173] And the way you protect yourself and your feelings turns out to be very important.

[174] So you quote Finnegan's Wake, mad props for that, James Joyce's book.

[175] I took a class in James Joyce in college.

[176] I think I read parts of Finningham Wake.

[177] I might have been on drugs or some kind, or I somehow got an A in that class, which probably refers to some kind of curve where nobody understood anything.

[178] The only thing I understood and really enjoyed is his short stories, The Dead.

[179] And then Ulysses, I kind of, I think read a few cliff notes that kind of got to the point.

[180] And Finnegan's Wake was just a hopeless person.

[181] It's a, you, for people who haven't looked at it, maybe you can elucidate to me better, but I felt like I was reading things, words, and the words made sense, like standing next to each other.

[182] But when you kind of read for a while, you realize you didn't actually understand anything that was said.

[183] Right.

[184] But did you have a feeling, though?

[185] That's one thing I found interesting about Finnegan's Week.

[186] I never fully understood it, but the words caused feelings in me, which I found fascinating.

[187] And sometimes I couldn't predict it from the semantic black and white context of what I was seeing in front of me on the page.

[188] But the rhythm or the melody would make me feel certain ways.

[189] And that was what I always was intrigued by with Joyce.

[190] Of course, that was his, he existed on a spectrum too.

[191] And he wrote, as you say, more accessible works.

[192] I learned a lot about Irish history from Portrait of the Artist as a Young Man. And there was just, he could be as objective as he wanted to be.

[193] But then when he let himself loose, he was in this realm where the words had their own purpose separate from, you know, semantic meaning from their dry, you know, dictionary definition.

[194] You know, there's a funny story that was told.

[195] It doesn't matter if it's true or not.

[196] But they said that James Joyce, when he was young, when he was in his teen years, would go around sort of violent drinking and so on and telling everybody that he, he's going to be one of, if not the greatest writers of the 20th century.

[197] And he turned out to be that.

[198] So I always think about that little story that somebody told me, because I have a lot of people come up to me, including myself.

[199] I'm a bit of a dreamer.

[200] You get into certain moods where you say, I'm going to be the greatest anything ever.

[201] You get like people tell you this, especially young people.

[202] And it kind of, It makes me feel all kinds of ways, but that story reminds me that, like, you just might be one of the greatest writers of the 21st century, for example, if somebody were to tell me that.

[203] And, like, don't immediately disregard that.

[204] Because one of the people that say that, that's almost like a pre -condition, that's a requirement, just to believe in yourself.

[205] Maybe it's not a full requirement, but it's an interesting story.

[206] Well, I think when someone tells you that, then it creates one sees an opportunity, and then it would be a tragedy if the opportunity weren't captured, right?

[207] And so then that creates some impetus, some motivation to do something good.

[208] I think the mind, it's like, you know, I guess that's what, like, books or books or whatever.

[209] I don't even know if it's a book the secret plugs into.

[210] They kind of make a whole industry out of it.

[211] But there is something about the mind, believing something, making it a reality.

[212] It is just time and time again with Steve Jobs, like your belief in yourself, your belief in an idea, sort of embracing the me versus the world, embracing the madness of this idea and making it a life pursuit.

[213] Somehow more's reality around, for some tiny fraction of the population, for everybody else, you descend into, you know, all the beautiful ways that failure materializes in our lifetime.

[214] You mentioned love earlier.

[215] I mean, that's a great example of how belief in something makes it real, right?

[216] It's not reasonable on the face of it, but because you believe it's reasonable, then it actually does become reasonable, and then it is real.

[217] And so that's a good example.

[218] That doesn't happen.

[219] I'm also in a bioengineering department.

[220] We don't imagine that a bridge is soundly built, and then it is soundly built.

[221] That's something that it doesn't come up in too many realms of human existence, but love is one of them, and the ability to have a fixed idea and to say it's true, and then it is true.

[222] A bridge is a kind of manifestation of love, so maybe it does work a little bit, but it can break down like Chernobyl did.

[223] You can't just say it's safe.

[224] You have to also prove it safe.

[225] But on Finnegan's Wake, I think, maybe correct me if I'm wrong, you're using kind of Finnegan's Wake to give one perspective on what madness is of what's going on in the mind.

[226] How much of that is that we're simply unable to communicate with the person on the other side of their mind?

[227] There's almost like a little person inside the brain and they have some circuitry that's used to communicate emotion, communicate ideas to the outside world, and there's something about that circuitry that makes it difficult to communicate with the little person on the other side.

[228] So if you look at what shows up in schizophrenia, with many cases what we call thought disorders, what we call the individual speech symptoms of schizophrenia, Finnegan's Wake is loaded with them, and it's just full of them.

[229] We talk about clang associations in schizophrenia, where the word that is said echoes in some way the previous word, and we call that a clang association, There's no other reason than the similarity of the sound, like a clang of a garage door being hit.

[230] And it has a, and sometimes it's not even a word, and we call that a neologism, a new word being created, and of course, Finnegan's Wake is full of that.

[231] And then we also, in schizophrenia, whether there's what we call loose associations or tangential thought processes, of course, full of that where things just go off in directions that are not linear or logical.

[232] And you can't read Finnegan's Wake, I think, without, certainly as a psychiatrist, you can't read it without thinking about schizophrenia.

[233] And then when we look at the families of people with schizophrenia, and Joyce was no exception, there very often are people within the family who are on the spectrum.

[234] Some have it.

[235] Some are able to see it from a distance, from a safe distance.

[236] There's an association between schizophrenia and what we call.

[237] schizetipal personality disorder where people are not quite in this severe state of schizophrenia but have some magical thinking have some unusual thought patterns very often those are family members of people with schizophrenia so this points to this again to this idea that that there is a range even along this very severe very genetic biological illness that human beings dwell on different spots along that spectrum i should mention that we have my friend surrogate pulling up stuff young surrogere old surrogate i don't know what to call you but uh there's drafts of finnigan's wake yeah i actually saw pictures of this from um i think it was on instagram or something these are early drafts of finnigan's way and it's so beautiful to see for people who are just listening there's just random paragraphs and writing all over the page with stuff crossed out and it's great to see that joyce himself was thinking in this kind of way as um as you're putting it together.

[238] How much do you think he was thinking about schizophrenia, the schizophrenic mind?

[239] I think a lot.

[240] I think, you know, it's known that his daughter suffered from schizophrenia, and this is what's depicted here on the page is something that I'm sure he either felt himself and some level was able to access this non -linearity of processing or had seen enough in family that he knew what it was and was able to reflect it down in black and white on the paper.

[241] So what he was able to do was quite authentic in that sense.

[242] Of course, I don't want to pigeonhole him.

[243] He was doing much more than that.

[244] It was much more than talking about altered human thought processes and thought disorders.

[245] But that was an aspect that he was so good at representing that it had to be intentional to some extent.

[246] A tiny tangent, what does your own writing look like for this book?

[247] Because it's extremely well written.

[248] How many edits did you just drink some whiskey and, like, I'm imagining Hemingway style?

[249] What's a very different, the writing is very different.

[250] I mean, it's really, really well written, which was like, I was reading it.

[251] It makes you realize, because I was expecting sort of a science kind of, which it is like, you know, lucidating something about the human mind kind of thing.

[252] But you could also probably write really strong.

[253] novels.

[254] So maybe that's in the future.

[255] But anyway, what is your, how many edits?

[256] How many, what's your style?

[257] Does it look like that?

[258] Is it more structured, organized?

[259] Unfortunately, I used a laptop, so I didn't have this sort of a beautiful record.

[260] No typewriter, cigarette, and whiskey.

[261] I did explore, I was, you know, which was there a particular altered state that would help me to be most creative.

[262] And I found actually, I actually did the best while, you know, sober, but slightly disinhibited in the late hours of the night or early morning.

[263] Yeah, particularly late hours of the night there.

[264] You know, I have a friend who would tell me that she thought that very early in the morning, her inner critic was still asleep and she could write more effectively before her inner critic woke up.

[265] And I actually found that outstanding advice for me that I often found that I was looser and could write more in the morning.

[266] But the other interesting thing is, is each chapter, each story, it's about a different human being with a different class of psychiatric disorder.

[267] That's what each story, each chapter is anchored in.

[268] But I'm trying to use words that and style of writing and addiction that captures the feeling of the disorder.

[269] And so it's different in each story.

[270] In the story about mania, which is a very expansive, exuberant, at least briefly uplifting state where the words come out in a torrent and they're complex and pressured and elaborate.

[271] I try to capture that feeling with the words used in that chapter.

[272] And then in the schizophrenia or psychosis chapter, where things slowly fragment over time and become looser and separated, I tried to capture that in the writing too.

[273] So for each, it wasn't as if there was a single mode I could be in for the whole book.

[274] For each chapter, I had to put myself into a different mode to capture that inner feeling of the disorder.

[275] When you put yourself in that mode, does that change you?

[276] Yeah.

[277] I couldn't turn it on and off right away.

[278] I had to, first I would start by thinking about the person or the people, one or two people based on real patients and the stories that are put forth.

[279] The symptom descriptions are real.

[280] They're from the patients.

[281] Of course, all details change to protect privacy, but the actual symptom descriptions are real.

[282] And I would sit with them and really try to inhabit the space of the mind of that person that I knew.

[283] And that's not instantaneous.

[284] It would take some time.

[285] I needed quiet.

[286] I need to be still.

[287] That's another reason.

[288] Late at night is good.

[289] Sergey posted that drowsiness gives creativity boosts according to Andrew Huberman.

[290] Thank you, Andrew.

[291] He's not wrong.

[292] Is that wrong?

[293] Why projections?

[294] Is it, I mean, there's a, instead of putting words into your mouth, because I can imagine a lot, I mean, to me, no, I will start putting words in your mouth despite what I just said.

[295] So, I mean, to me, projections, working on neural networks, for example, from an artificial neural networks, from a machine learning perspective, it's often, that's exactly what you're doing.

[296] You have an incredibly complex thing, and you're trying to find simple representation, in order for you to make sense of it.

[297] So I was kind of thinking about in that way, which is like this incredibly complex neuronal network that is kind of projecting itself onto the world through this low bandwidth expression of emotion and speech and all that kind of stuff.

[298] And the way it's, we only have that window into your soul, the eyes and the speech and so on.

[299] So in that way where when there's, any kind of disorder, we get to only see that disorder through that narrow window as opposed to the full complexity of its origins.

[300] The word projections definitely serves that purpose here, but it's got a few other really appropriate other connotations as well.

[301] So the first thing is a projection in terms of neuroscience is this long -range connection that goes from one part of the brain to another.

[302] And so it's what binds two parts of our brain together.

[303] There are projections, long -range, connections of axons, these are the outgoing threads that connect one part of the brain to another part.

[304] There's a projection that links, for example, auditory cortex where we hear things to reward centers where we can feel, where feelings of pleasure and reward are initiated.

[305] And it's been shown that if you have reduced connectivity along that dimension, you are less able to enjoy music.

[306] And so these connections, these projections matter.

[307] They define how effectively two parts of the brain can engage with each other and join together to form a joint representation of something.

[308] So that's one meaning.

[309] It's pure neuroscience.

[310] The word projection is used all the time, and it happens to be something that optogenetics, a technique that maybe we'll talk about a little later, it works particularly well with.

[311] We can use light to turn on or off the activity along these projections from one spot of the brain to another.

[312] And this is particularly referring to the long range connections.

[313] It's particularly straightforward along these long range projections that connect different parts of the brain.

[314] But it works over shorter range too.

[315] But then there's this other meaning of projections which you were bringing up, which is very relevant, which is at some point you can reduce something from one level of dimensionality to another.

[316] You can project down into a lower dimensional space, for example.

[317] And then finally there's a psychiatric term projections, which comes up all the time, which is we very often will look at our internal states and to understand somebody else, we'll project them onto somebody else.

[318] We'll try to understand someone else's behavior and make sense of it by projecting our own inner feelings, our own sort of narrative onto them, and use that as a way to help us understand them better.

[319] And we'll do the reverse too.

[320] We'll take things we see in the outside world and we'll bring them into ourselves and see how well they map, how will they align.

[321] That's called introjection.

[322] So projections as it turns out to be a really rich word.

[323] Then finally, of course, there's the very common sense of it as a projector that illuminates by conveying information across space with light.

[324] So for English, for English language, perfect word to use for this book.

[325] But what was funny is not every, there are a lot of international translations now, and all those rich connotations aren't captured in other languages.

[326] And so for some translations, connections is used instead of projections.

[327] In fact, even in England, the British version is connections instead of projections because apparently projections doesn't have the full connotation I was told.

[328] So you have to sacrifice some of the rich ambiguity of meaning with connections.

[329] That's interesting.

[330] I mean, could I, man, words are so interesting.

[331] They could have so many of me. I love language and how much is lost in translation.

[332] I'm very fortunate enough to be able to speak.

[333] I'm not good at languages.

[334] I was just, I guess, forced to buy less.

[335] circumstance to learn two languages, Russian and English, and it's just so interesting to watch how much of culture, how much of people, how much a history is lost in translation.

[336] The poetry, the music, the history, the pain, the way the scientists actually express themselves, which is funny.

[337] I mean, it's so sad to see how much brilliant work that was written in Russian.

[338] there's a whole culture of science in the Soviet Union that is not lost.

[339] It makes me wonder in the modern day how much incredible science is going on in China that is lost in translation.

[340] And I'll never, I mean, that makes me very sad because I'll never learn Chinese in the same way that I've learned English and Russian.

[341] Maybe whenever I say stuff like that, people are like, well, there's still time, but you know, yeah, that's, that's, that's actually fair that I think the 21st century both China and US will have very important roles in the scientific development and we should actually bridge the gap through language and that that doesn't just mean convincing Chinese to speak English that means also learning Chinese well we need these bridge people who can do both you know the you know Nabokov for example writing in English beautifully you know one of my favorite it's Borges, who mentioned earlier, he wrote both in English and in Spanish.

[342] I think, you know, beautifully in both.

[343] We need those people who can serve as bridges across cultures.

[344] He really can do both.

[345] You mentioned Borges.

[346] So you open your book with a few lines from a poem by Jorge Luis Borges, a love poem.

[347] I'm going to read parts of it because it's a damn good poem.

[348] It's called Two English Poems.

[349] I mean, there's, I'd like to understand why you used it and the specific parts you used, which is interesting.

[350] But then when I read the full thing, so I think you used it as a sort of beautiful description of what it means to delve deep into understanding, offering yourself to the task of understanding another human being.

[351] But if you look at the full context of the poem, it's also a damn good description of being hit by love and overtaking by it and sort of trying to figure out how to make sense of the world now that you've been stricken by it.

[352] It says a bunch of things about chatting and significantly with friends and all those kinds of things.

[353] And then the poem reads, The Big Wave brought you.

[354] This is the moment, I guess, of the universe where the two people you fall in love.

[355] Maybe I'm totally misreading this poem, by the way.

[356] It doesn't matter.

[357] You can't misread the poem.

[358] So it goes on, words, any words, your laughter, and you so lazily and incessantly beautiful.

[359] We talked, and you have forgotten the words.

[360] The shattering dawn finds me in a deserted street of my city.

[361] Your profile turned away.

[362] The sounds that go to make your name.

[363] The lilt of your laughter.

[364] These are the illustrious toys you have left me. So these little memories of these peculiar little details.

[365] He remembers.

[366] Those are the illustrious toys.

[367] I apologize to mix my own words with the poem, but you should definitely read it.

[368] I turn them over in the dawn.

[369] I lose them.

[370] I find them.

[371] I tell them to the few stray dogs and to the few stray stars of the dawn.

[372] Your dark, rich life.

[373] I must get at you somehow.

[374] I put away those illustrious toys you have left me. I want your hidden look, your real smile.

[375] That lonely, mocking smile, your cool mirror knows.

[376] I want your hidden look, your real smile.

[377] So this is the first part of the poem, and then it goes on, which is some of the parts that you reference.

[378] Second part is, what can I hold you with?

[379] I offer you lean streets, desperate sunset, the moon of the jagged suburbs.

[380] I offer you the bitterness of a man who has looked long and long in a lonely moon.

[381] I offer you my ancestors, My dead men, the ghosts that living men have honored in bronze.

[382] My father's father killed in the frontier of Buenos Aires, two bullets through his lungs and so on, so on.

[383] I offer you whatever insights my books may hold, whatever manliness of humor in my life.

[384] I offer you the loyalty of a man who has never been loyal.

[385] I offer you that kernel of myself that I have saved somehow the central heart that deals not in words, traffics, not with dreams, and is untouched by time, my joy, and adversities.

[386] And I think this is the part they include in the book, I offer you the memory of a yellow rose seen as sunset, years before you were born.

[387] I, damn, that's a good line.

[388] Okay, I offer you explanations of yourself, theories about yourself, authentic and surprising news of yourself.

[389] I can give you my loneliness, my darkness, the hunger of my heart.

[390] I'm trying to bribe you with uncertainty, to with danger, with defeat.

[391] That is a man who's in love and longing.

[392] If taken, but I just wanna go back to, maybe you could say why you wanna include that poem, but also your dark, rich life, I must get at you somehow.

[393] I put away those illustrious toys you have left me out.

[394] I want your hidden look, your real smile, that lonely, mocking smile, your cool mirror nose.

[395] Well, sometimes I meet a stranger, and I just, it's like a double take.

[396] It's like, who are you?

[397] Have we met before somewhere?

[398] Who's that person behind there?

[399] And I want to get at that, whatever that is.

[400] And of course, maybe that's what love is because maybe that's the whole pursuit, like a lifelong pursuit of getting at that person.

[401] Maybe that's what that is.

[402] And like that insatiable sort of curiosity to keep getting to.

[403] Like, well, who's that person in your own private life?

[404] Yeah.

[405] So that, absolutely, I think that it was a beautiful description of what you just said when there's that first moment and then you want to dive deeper.

[406] You want to know what the, what the hidden mysteries are.

[407] In a way, it's a love poem.

[408] As a scientist, though, it also, it's a bit of how a scientist can love science.

[409] And that wanting to dive deeper is, it's almost like, again, where it could be a love affair with investigating the human mind, for example.

[410] And that was one reason it spoke to me also, again, thinking about the broader sweep of where the human mind came from, the steps it took to get where it is today, what was given up along the way, what compromises were made.

[411] And here's where the darkness of the poem starts to come in a little bit, too.

[412] It doesn't shy away from the negativity, from the confusion, from the danger.

[413] and then at the very end the board face is offering up scenes from his life parts of himself and this is how we connect with people we offer up parts of ourselves just here it is and then we see how well does that map onto what you have and it's that offering up that I liked and not the good stuff or not only the good stuff the yellow rose is nice but he's offering up the bad stuff too and that to me was important for the book because I'm offering up hard stuff too in fact a lot of it and also hard stuff from within me from my own personal side too and that was there's a lot of vulnerability that comes with that that comes with love that comes with writing you have to be open you have to be vulnerable and so I thought that reflected what I was trying to do and I thought it was as an epigraphic kind of made it clear how vulnerable I was in taking this step but also So what could come out of it?

[414] And also in a meta way, because I was not familiar with this poem, it may be curious of the poem itself to pull at that thread of finding out more.

[415] You picked a very particular part that kind of made you want to pull it that thread and see where did these few lines come from?

[416] because I read it as a curiosity of a scientist, those lines alone.

[417] And also as a desperate human being searching, like offering himself for an understanding or connection with another human being.

[418] And then, because I wasn't sure if it's a love poem or not, or if it's desperation or if it's curiosity, whatever it is, and then you see the love poem.

[419] I mean, I don't know.

[420] That's going to stick with me for a while, your dark, rich life, and then a few lines in here are just, I mean, those are, I'm going to just use them as pickup lines at a bar.

[421] I offer you the memory of a yellow rose seen at sunset years before you were born.

[422] Now, that's a pickup line I've never heard, if I've ever heard one.

[423] Anyway, sorry.

[424] But this is universal.

[425] You know, you see it in so many forms of art, you know, Like, you know, we're in Texas, now you see this in country, country and western songs.

[426] It's often a list of things.

[427] Like, here's how I describe myself.

[428] There's this and there's that and there's the other thing.

[429] And here you are, these things matter to me. And I hope they matter to you too.

[430] It's a pretty universal form.

[431] But he did it in this very artful and very vulnerable way.

[432] It was both beautiful and you could feel the hurt coming from him too, and that was important.

[433] The dark stuff, too.

[434] I offer you my ancestors, my dead men, the ghosts that living men have honored in bronze and talking about two bullets through his lungs, bearded and dead, wrapped by his soldiers in the hide of a cow.

[435] My mother's grandfather, just 24, heading a charge of 300 men in Peru, now ghosts on vanished horses.

[436] So all of it, the whole history of it.

[437] Since it is a love poem, what do you think about love?

[438] Carl what's the role of love and the human condition we'll talk about the dark stuff but maybe love is the dark stuff too I mean it's it's the most powerful connection we can form and that's that's what it makes it so important to us it's the strongest and most stable connection that we can form with another person and that matters immensely it matters for for the human family to have evolved to be something that could survive against the odds that that that we've faced over the years of that unreasonable bond that becomes reasonable by virtue of its own existence and of course that joy the wild raw joy of love is not a bad thing either so you put these together the four the strongest bridge we can form and the reward and the joy that it brings that's that's what love is to me and from you know my perspective this is something that you know it can be hard to capture fairly because you want to talk about the positive and the negative sides at once they need to be wrapped up together for a full honest description of what it is and that that's hard to do in a in a compact form and so you have to take time to talk about love you have to take time to to do it justice um it takes a book or at least a poem.

[439] Or several thousands of them.

[440] I don't know.

[441] So could you pull up, there's a video I saw.

[442] Yeah, like right here.

[443] So can you pause for a second?

[444] So there's March of the Penguins.

[445] So you always see like penguins huddling together against, I mean, sorry if I see just metaphors and everything, but them huddling together against the harshness of the conditions around them.

[446] That's very kind of...

[447] That's like a metaphor for life, like finding this connection.

[448] That's kind of what love is.

[449] It's like it allows you to forget whatever the absurdity, whatever the suffering of life is together, you get to like huddle for warmth.

[450] And that's why I love the sort of just the honesty and the intensity of the way penguins just in the middle of like the cold do this.

[451] And then this video I saw a lonely, this is misinformation.

[452] So the name of the video is Lonely deranged Penguin.

[453] I don't know if he's deranged.

[454] So if you play it, so he left his pack and there's a nice voiceover and you don't need to play it.

[455] But he, for some reason, left the pack and journeyed out into the mountains.

[456] And so the narrator says that he's deranged.

[457] He's lost his mind.

[458] Now, I'd like to project the idea that he's actually, there's so many stories he could think of.

[459] he's returning to his homeland he's an outsider thinking journeying out into the unknown thinking he may be able to discover something greater than the tribe he might be looking for lost love why is he deranged immediately why is he lost his mind anyway but this people should look up this video because to me i might be the only one who uh romanticizes this but it's such a nice kind of uh it's both a picture of perhaps a mental disorder which is what the video kind of describes and it may be some deeper explanation that's not that that has to do with the motivation of a of a mind but yeah i don't know if i don't know if you have a deeper analysis on this penguin well i like you the psychiatrist i would i would want to sit down with a penguin and go through i want to see the notes from his prior therapist and but this this actually is relevant, not knowing what was that penguin's motivation.

[460] We have very clear situations where there are, both within an individual, we go through periods of time when we stay in one place and we reap the benefits from what we've built, and then we go through periods of foraging, of wandering.

[461] Even if there may be resources where we are, we have periods of time in our lives where we wander, where we go in an exploratory mode, and different people express that trait in different ways.

[462] This is not a human -specific trait.

[463] If you go down to the tiny little nematode worm, sea elegans with 302 nervous system cells, they go through these phases of foraging and rest, and different individuals have different propensity to forage or to rest and stay in one place.

[464] At the level of the species, that's really good, that there's that diversity.

[465] in their willingness to forage.

[466] Some stay where they are.

[467] The species is somewhat on a firm footing then.

[468] But some carry a burden, a risk for themselves, but it's good for the species that they are explorers and they will venture out.

[469] The migration patterns that different species blunder into and that turn out to be really good, they weren't logically derived.

[470] They most certainly started from something like this, an exploration.

[471] Humans do this too, you think?

[472] And we do it too.

[473] In fact, it's something we do extremely well.

[474] Let's talk about psychiatry a little bit.

[475] So in my book, you're a rock star.

[476] First of all, for people who don't know, aside from the neurological view of the brain and neuroscience view of the brain, you're also one of the great psychiatrists of our time.

[477] I've always, not always, but when I was younger, I dreamed about being a psychiatrist.

[478] so it's like getting to meet your heroes and also you know getting to meet the people who the best at the top of the world at the thing you've failed to pursue so there's I'm getting a free therapy session on top of that okay so what big picture what is the practice the goal the hope of modern psychiatry if you could try to describe the discipline and as you see it, maybe historically throughout the 20th century in contrasting to what it is today.

[479] Or maybe if you want to describe to what you hope psychiatry becomes or longs to become in the 21st century.

[480] It's been an interesting journey.

[481] Psychiatry started out pretty firmly grounded in neurology and pathology.

[482] Some of the initial founders effectively of the field were very well grounded in microscopy.

[483] looking at cells, working with patients, particularly on the neurological side, and this certainly included Freud and some of his contemporaries.

[484] But they rapidly discovered that what they could work with at the level of cells and microscopy was so far from the realm of what they could get from a human being.

[485] And what they were getting from the human being was so much more interesting and had so mysterious and so unknown that many of them just said we're going to inhabit this domain and we're going to work with the people with their words and understand what we can based on verbal communication because that was the only tool that people really had.

[486] And that was a very important step for the field.

[487] I would say one of the interesting things that came from the early decades of psychiatry really was this distinction between the conscious and the unconscious mind and paying particular attention to the unconscious mind is something that was worthy of consideration that might be important in explaining people's actions and that perhaps even insight into that was valuable in its own right.

[488] And out of that, psychoanalysis became a practice that was not always focused on cures or treatment, but was more focused on insight.

[489] What does it mean?

[490] How can we help people understand why they're feeling something or thinking something or dreaming something?

[491] And that insight, separate even from treatment, was an interesting thing.

[492] As long as one was honest about that and said, you know, we're going for understanding, we're going for insight.

[493] Maybe it's So just pause on that.

[494] If you look at the father of psychoanalysis, Zygman Freud, what do you make of the ideas that he had?

[495] So you mentioned taking the unconscious, the subconscious seriously.

[496] That's like step one, like that there could be worlds we do not have direct access for, and we probe with them through conversation.

[497] Or is that too simplistic to call psychoanalysis conversation?

[498] That's not too simplistic.

[499] But that's right.

[500] And I think that was valuable.

[501] Where Freud ended up breaking from some of his contemporaries, he was very focused on this unconscious as being so tightly linked to libido.

[502] And really, from his perspective, you couldn't really separate the operation of the unconscious mind from these aspects of the libidinous aspects.

[503] And that was one reason.

[504] You know, sexual, sexually related, you know, drives.

[505] Carl Jung, who was his, you know, you know, contemporary, that's one factor that led to them separating was, you know, Carl Jung felt there was a lot more to the unconscious than this libidinous aspect of it.

[506] And he saw it as a much more complete, alternate representation of the conscious self, one that maybe reflected a whole range of different motivations and desires.

[507] to properly treat it when had to consider all of them rather than the ones that Freud was hooked.

[508] Call Young's shadow.

[509] Thank you for the high level of images that Sergei is pulling up.

[510] For people who are just listening, he pulled up a, as a quote from Segment Freud's a meme, your mom quote, Freud.

[511] So the shadow, the Carl Jung's shadow, encompasses everything, not just the desire to have sex with your mother or sex, period.

[512] that's right that's right if you look at those two folks on mass i mean there's a kind of it's almost like a technique for philosophical exploration of human mind human motivations so it's not even like necessarily it's also doubles as a methodology for helping people but it's almost like a it's a kind of philosophical method right this is the fascinating thing about about psychoanalysis and it even though it's it's I would say mostly not considered a treatment today it persists for a couple of reasons one is it's it's thought that it gives people some insight but second there's been a huge influence on on literature on philosophy on art and the opening up of discussion about what was below our conscious mind was so so fertile in the implications that it sort of reverberated and still does throughout all these different realms of human endeavor from different artistic, you know, experiences that people have can be colored by this concept of the unconscious.

[513] Now, the other thing that was interesting is, is this distinction, you know, what are the parts of the unconscious?

[514] And so there were these id and ego and super ego subdivisions that, you know, that Freud, for example, would talk about them.

[515] And the id was the primary, the primal drives that an infant would have or that a very young child just warmth and feeding and then later the sexual or libidinous aspects.

[516] And for Freud, the later happened very quickly.

[517] that's the controversial thing about him I think I guess he thought like even children had sexual desires that they're like dealing with contending with so it's the full thing hungry wanting to eat wanting to poop wanting to have sex yeah and he was extremely focused on that on that aspect but then there was then there was this the superego which brought on these later sort of moralistic uh sort of codes of conduct and and that of course was very often and intention, but all this could play out subconsciously.

[518] And then the ego, this third aspect, was mediating, and for its conception, mediated this tension between the different parts.

[519] Now, I think that's interesting.

[520] I will say that in some ways, it's maybe unnecessary from the perspective of modern neuroscience to divide things up that way from the, you know, the moralistic drives and the primal gratification drives.

[521] In some ways, they're all drives, and maybe they're even all primal drives.

[522] You know, the moralistic drives, they're taught, and they're taught in ways that ultimately relate back to survival, and you could even say selfish aspects of health in life for the self and family.

[523] And so this is, I don't, I think it's maybe an artificial distinction.

[524] The concept of the unconscious is very valuable and very interesting.

[525] But these categorizations of id and super ego may not map onto neurobiology in any particular way.

[526] There's a town hall of competing drives and desires and they interrelate to each other.

[527] They involve different aspects of the brain.

[528] and the history of the person and actions and choices come out of the result of that overall shouting in the town hall.

[529] So in some sense, Carl Jung was a step into the direction of liberating yourself from such harsh categorizations.

[530] Do you think, I mean, you have Daniel Kahneman with System 1 and System 2, there's just these very compelling categorizations of the human mind that seem to be sticky in our, in the super ego, no, in the, you know, how we talk about these ideas and so on.

[531] Do you think those are helpful or did they get in the way is it some kind of balance in terms of deeper understanding of how the mind actually works?

[532] You know, it's from modern neuroscience, whenever we seem to get closer to addressing a question like this at the level of cells, it seems to get farther away.

[533] And I'll give you an example of what I mean by that.

[534] So one thing I'm doing in my laboratory and many people are doing is we are listening in on the activity of cells, neurons, in the brain of mice or rats or fish or monkeys.

[535] Individual cells.

[536] Individual cells, exactly, of which there are, you know, in our brain many billions.

[537] And when we do, and we try to predict what action will be taken by an animal, to address this question, where does the choice arise?

[538] where does the impetus to make a particular selection of one action versus another action, where does that start in the brain?

[539] If you're recording, listening in on the activity of cells all across the brain, where is the earliest spot you can pick up a choice being made?

[540] Well, at one level you might think how excited would Young have been to see this or Freud or the early psychoanalyst to see where this starts.

[541] But it's not so simple.

[542] because an emerging theme in very recent neuroscience, literally over the last few years, is that things sort of all start together, all across the brain.

[543] And so you can be recording from the cortex, this rim of cells at the surface of the brain, or you can be recording deeper in a structure called the striatum, which is a little older.

[544] It's more tightly linked to action.

[545] And then structures called the thalamus, other parts of the brain.

[546] And if you record from these, these all sort of represent the action and the choice more or less all at about the same time, very close.

[547] And so you can't point to a particular spot and say here's where the choice or the action originates.

[548] It's a group.

[549] Finding the free will in neuron.

[550] It's relevant to that question.

[551] Nobody is close to being able to point to such a thing.

[552] Well, close is a relative term.

[553] And nobody, what I tweet today, all generalizations are wrong.

[554] So including this one.

[555] Let's actually talk about that.

[556] So the study of individual cells, if you can linger on your sense that as you get closer to that understanding, it feels like you're getting farther away.

[557] Why is that?

[558] Because that often is the feeling until you're actually there.

[559] So like, you know, that's a thing.

[560] thought when I'm running, and I know there's only a mile left, it just feels like that mile is just getting longer and longer, but eventually you finish.

[561] So maybe we're getting close to cracking open these like beginnings of ascending, like we'll talk about consciousness or these very difficult, big questions about the human mind.

[562] Where do they start?

[563] You're right to say we shouldn't generalize or make absoluteest statements.

[564] But I would say right now, the reason things are looking even harder to crack than we had initially thought, we now have the data streams that we've wanted for so long in terms of activity patterns all across the brain at the level of cells.

[565] We can literally see what cells are doing.

[566] Immense data sets, you know, we get, these are, you know, time series of one individual cell with, you know, sub -second resolution, and you can collect this from, you know, enormous numbers of cells across the brain.

[567] So very rich data sets that we've wanted for a long time, and yet having these has not led to an understanding of truly where actions initiate in terms of regions or locations.

[568] I'll ask you a few questions on that.

[569] Is the answer, high -level question by your intuition, is the answer within the data?

[570] Or do we need different kind of data?

[571] So we should also say that when you collect data about the brain, there's like the richness of information you're collecting, but there's also human doing stuff.

[572] Like, and information, so static information about the human and dynamic information about the human, and you can get them to do different stuff, and you can select different humans, and that's part of the collection of data aspect.

[573] So, like, when you're collecting data about the brain, there's some truths that you can, you know, in machine learnings like annotations like supervised learning, there's some true things you can hold on to before you look at the full rich mess complexity of the human mind.

[574] So given the data you've looked at, do you think the answer for the origin of free will in the human mind can be found?

[575] well one one amazing thing is that nobody's nobody's found it but we have these rich data sets and and then there's a conundrum which is is it in the data and we just don't know how to look at it maybe we don't know the right scale the right projection to make of the data the right way to interpret it and here's where causal testing becomes very valuable because then instead of just passively observing, well, here are the activity patterns, and then here's the choice made by the animal, as we've gotten more powerful at reaching in and causing things to happen in the brain, turning up or down the activity of certain types of cells or defined populations of cells and seeing how that affects actions.

[576] These causal perturbations have turned out to be very valuable.

[577] We're just now getting to the point where we can apply these in very wide swaths of the brain at cellular resolution, and so we're going to be able, hopefully, to make some headway on this question with causality.

[578] And those, that's the one thing that optogenetics provides us this way of using light that we develop to control cells.

[579] This is an untapped, relatively untapped at this broad brain -wide scale, and hopefully we can get there in the near future.

[580] But I would say that the answer may be in the data, but we don't know how to find it.

[581] Well, there's this interactive element where you can cause stuff that's really powerful because you get to, I mean, as opposed to collecting data passively, you're collecting data actively.

[582] So can you maybe describe one of the many things you're known for?

[583] One of the big things is called optogenetics.

[584] What is it?

[585] Optogenetics is a way of causing things to happen.

[586] It's a way of determining what actually matters in terms of the activity of the brain for the amazing things.

[587] It does sensation, cognition, action.

[588] And what it does is it provides activity.

[589] It's a way of playing in, if you will, activity patterns into precisely defined cells.

[590] And the way we do it is pretty cool, I think.

[591] It's, you know, right away there's a problem.

[592] If you think about how do we do this?

[593] How could we play in well -defined activity patterns and provide a stream of activity into this cell and that cell and that cell, but not these other cells?

[594] So just for context, we're talking about the brains of mice, monkeys, humans, and then the goal is to try to control accurately the behavior of a single neuron, and then to be able to monitor single collection of single neurons to then say, well, to draw some deeper insight about the origins, first of all, the function of different parts of the brain, different neurons, different kinds of neurons, but also the origins of the big things, the flap of the butterfly wing that leads to an actual behavioral thing.

[595] Yeah.

[596] So if you could, exactly, so if you could turn on or off the brain or parts of the brain, brain or cell types or individual cells at the natural rate and rhythm and timing of normal brain activity, that would be immensely valuable because you could determine what actually mattered, what could cause complex things to happen, and what could prevent complex things from happening in a specific way.

[597] But right away, you've got a problem if you want to do this, and scientists, neuroscientists have wanted to do this for a long time.

[598] Francis Crick of double helix of DNA fame, he wrote a famous page.

[599] in 1999, he got interested in neuroscience later in life, and he said, what we need in neuroscience is a way that we could turn on or off the activity of individual types of neurons in a behaving animal.

[600] And he even said the ideal signal would be light, because it would be fast, it could penetrate through the brain to some extent.

[601] And he had no idea how to do it.

[602] He said this would probably be very far -fetched, but it would be a good thing.

[603] And so that's what you're actually saying.

[604] Like, if you want to do this kind of thing, and then you imagine, like, how do I get inside the brain?

[605] It's pretty difficult to do it.

[606] It's pretty difficult.

[607] And then even once you get in, it's hard because all brain cells are electrical.

[608] All neurons are electrically activated.

[609] And so if you wanted to use electricity as what you were putting in, you won't have any specificity at all.

[610] If you have an electrode, a wire, and you put it in the brain, and you send current through it, all the cells near the electrode will be stimulated.

[611] That's like trying to control fish by spraying them with water.

[612] Yeah, right, because there's already a lot of electricity going around anyway, and you're adding more.

[613] But there's no specificity, even among the different kinds of cells either, because all around the wire that you've put in, there are going to be so many different cells doing totally different things.

[614] Many of them in opposition to each other, we know that's one way the brain is set up.

[615] There are parts of the brain where neurons side by side are doing completely different things and maybe even antagonistic to each other.

[616] So what do you do?

[617] How do you play in activity with any kind of specificity?

[618] Well, what you do is use, what we found is what you can do is make some cells responsive to light.

[619] Now, normally, no cells deep in the brain really respond to light.

[620] They're not built for that.

[621] There's no reason for them to respond to light in there, which is a great situation to start with, because any light sensitivity you can provide to some cells will be a huge signal above the noise.

[622] and so that's that's what we do with optogenetics we take genes bits of DNA from microbes single -celled organisms and these single -celled organisms like algae they make little proteins that sit in the surface of their cells that receive light capture a photon of light and open up a little hole in the membrane of the cell and let charge particles ions like sodium and potassium flow across the membrane of the cell.

[623] And that, these algae and bacteria, they do this for their own reasons because that helps them move.

[624] It helps them make and use energy.

[625] But that's a beautiful thing for neuroscience because movement of ions charge particles across the membrane of the cell is exactly the kind of electricity that neurons work with.

[626] So if we can take this bit of DNA that encodes this beautiful protein that turns light into electricity from algae, and if we can put it into some neurons, but not other neurons, which we can do using genetic tricks, then you've got a situation, then you can shine on the light, and only the cells that have the gene and that are expressing the gene will be the initial direct cells that are activated by the light.

[627] And so that's the essence of optogenetics is the ability to do that.

[628] We get that initial specificity that you could never get with an electrode.

[629] So let me say that this is, you recently got the Alaska Prize for this.

[630] It's a brilliant idea.

[631] So I talked to Andrew Huberman, who's a friend of yours, a friend of mine, not to jinx things, but he believes that you deserve the Nobel Prize for this.

[632] So I do too, but what my votes.

[633] Anyway, the thing is, it doesn't matter, prizes.

[634] be all forgotten.

[635] All of us will be forgotten.

[636] One of the cool ideas, cool idea is a cool idea.

[637] It's a really powerful idea.

[638] It's actually the origins of it you might be interested in are even, are very deep.

[639] There was a botanist in St. Petersburg named Andre Faminson.

[640] In 1866, he published a paper on the single -celled green algae.

[641] And he was the botanist who first noticed that they moved in response to light.

[642] These are tiny single -celled algae that have flagella, so they swim through the water.

[643] and he noticed this, he was a botanist, and he published this.

[644] It was a paper, you know, he wrote in German, but he published it in a French journal, and he was doing it from St. Petersburg, so it was a very international effort.

[645] But you have to go back to 1866, and that, I like to highlight how far back that discovery goes is back to Andre Finlinson.

[646] And this is a, it highlights the value of just pure basic science, That always originates somewhere in the Eastern European block.

[647] But I don't think he expected the splicing of genetic material from the algae into the human brain.

[648] And one of the cool things we've been able to do now with modern methods is to really study these proteins.

[649] And so we've discovered some of these proteins.

[650] Other groups have as well.

[651] We've dived deep into their structure, just like the double helix structure of DNA was uncovered with X -ray crystallography.

[652] We used the same method, x -ray crystallography, to see how these.

[653] beautiful little proteins work.

[654] We re -engineered them for all kinds of function.

[655] We can make them, instead of responding to blue light, we can make them respond to red light.

[656] We can speed them up, slow them down.

[657] We can make them with genetic engineering.

[658] We can make them have different ions flow through them.

[659] And so it's this convergence, as you said, like the botanist in 1866 couldn't have predicted what we could do with this.

[660] And the fact that we've been able to discover how these beautiful proteins work and then apply them to neuroscience is really a thrilling story.

[661] Is it possible to achieve scale, do you think, with this?

[662] Meaning, like, greater, like, what is the progress of the next 50 years, 100 years looks like, in terms of the precision and the scale of control of using light?

[663] It's going so fast, it's hard to predict.

[664] I'll give you a sense of it, though.

[665] In, you know, first paper, you know, we published in 2005, that was just in cultured neurons, By 2007, so that was in a dish.

[666] By 2007, we had it working in behaving mice.

[667] By 2009, we had it pretty general, so we had methods to really make it a versatile method.

[668] It could be applied to essentially any cell.

[669] By 2012, we could get to single cell resolution.

[670] We used light guidance strategies to target individual cells in the brain of a living mouse.

[671] By 2019, we were able to control up to 20 to 50 to 5.

[672] 50 individually specified single cells in the brain of a mouse and in ways that specifically changed its behavior that could bias its decisions one way or the other.

[673] In fact, we could take a mouse and without any visual stimulus at all, we could make it act as if it had seen a particular visual stimulus by playing in using the single cell resolution optogenetics a specific pattern of activity into 20 or 25 individually specified cells.

[674] That's 2019.

[675] To your question of scale, now in 2022, we're controlling hundreds of individually specified single cells over all a visual cortex of a mouse, all the part of the of the brain that is the initial direct target of the incoming information from the retina.

[676] Are you constrained to specific types of cells currently?

[677] Like you mentioned long range, it's a little easier.

[678] Is there constraints on which cells?

[679] Now there really isn't, now that we have this individual cell guidance, we can target any individual kind of cell very reliably.

[680] And so now to your question of scale, how far can we go?

[681] Well, things are moving quickly.

[682] It's hard to say.

[683] We can access individual cells across the entire brain now.

[684] If you look 10, 20 years in the future, you know, I think we'll surprise ourselves.

[685] but the fact that we're already able to cause specific perceptions to happen and specific actions means we're essentially where we want to be, and now it's a matter of just, you know, more experiments, more discoveries, but the basic principles are clear now.

[686] The basic capability is there.

[687] Is there a pathway to doing the same for humans?

[688] Optogenetics is primarily, it's a discovery tool that really is well suited for use in mice and rats, and monkeys because it involves putting in a gene and also delivering light.

[689] And those are two things that you can do in human beings, but you'd want to do in a very careful way.

[690] Now that said, there is actually just less than a year ago, my friend Botan Raska in Switzerland, he did the first human optogenetics therapy.

[691] And he published this in the journal Nature Medicine.

[692] So about 10, 12 years ago, he and I published a paper together where we gave him one of our optogenetic tools, one of these light -activated regulators of ion flow.

[693] These are called microbial opsons, by the way, opsons.

[694] And he put one of those into an extracted retina from a human being who had died.

[695] So it was a cadaveric retina.

[696] And he was able to show that optical control in this paper was able to turn on or off individual cells in the human retina.

[697] So that was a while back.

[698] He spent about 10 years of going through all the regulatory, you know, hoops and hurdles and going through primate studies.

[699] And finally, he was able to take a human being with a retinal degeneration syndrome, so someone who was blind in both eyes.

[700] and he gave one of these options into one eye of this human being who was blind and with the goal of conferring light sensitivity onto this retina that was not able to see light and he was able to make this person see through that eye so he took a blind person and the blind person could see now could reach for objects selectively on a table and he published this in nature medicine and it was, you know, that's an amazing thing.

[701] Do you know the title of the paper?

[702] What's his name again?

[703] Rosca, R -O -S -K -A.

[704] Can you look up the nature paper?

[705] Yeah, nature medicine.

[706] Nature medicine.

[707] So that's sort of proof of principle.

[708] Now the retina is very accessible.

[709] It's near the surface.

[710] You can use natural light or you can use brighter natural light.

[711] I'm myself, I see optogenetics as a discovery tool.

[712] It's a way to figure out the principles by which the brain works and how it operates.

[713] Partial recovery of visual function in a blind patient after optogenetic therapy.

[714] So he went to the full process of doing primates and then going, well, that's dedication, and that's really exciting to see.

[715] As beautiful as that is, and I'm glad he did all that work, there are so many other ways that optogenetics could help with therapies.

[716] Once you know the principles, then any kind of therapy can become more powerful.

[717] know the causal cells in a symptom like in lack of motivation or inability to enjoy things or altered sleep or altered energy.

[718] Once you know the cells that are causal, then you can make medications that address those cells.

[719] You could address brain stimulation treatments that might address those cells.

[720] Also diagnosis, you may be a very effective, systematic way of diagnosing, or at least providing you rich data to some of these deep questions about.

[721] about schizophrenia, about bipolar, all those kinds of things that are, the tools are low resolution currently for determining the degree to which you have a thing and whether you have a thing at all.

[722] Yeah, and exactly.

[723] And so the hope is, you know, that's a great example of how you can cure, or you can provide some relief for a symptom of a person who has a serious degenerative disease.

[724] But the principles are what we're after.

[725] And that's why I spend, even though I'm a psychiatrist, even though I still see patients, I'm not myself trying to drive any clinical trials in the lab.

[726] I'm trying to discover, and then any kind of therapy could result from that.

[727] What do you think about my friend Elon Musk and his efforts with NeuroLink?

[728] So this is another, there's a lot of things to say here, because there's a lot of ideas under the umbrella of neuralink.

[729] But one of them is to use electrical signals to stimulate, and then you also record, you collect electrical signals from the brain at a higher and higher resolution, and you go implant surgically the methods by which you do the stimulation and the data collection.

[730] So it's possible for the ideas of optogenetics to play well with this.

[731] And we can even zoom out outside of just neuralink and just the whole idea of brain computer interfaces.

[732] What are your thoughts?

[733] Well, I think the engineering that they've done is actually pretty cool.

[734] So I like the robots.

[735] Yeah, from the design perspective, and it was a design approach that wasn't being taken in academia.

[736] And it's great that they did it, and I think it's pretty cool.

[737] So I'll say that.

[738] Also, there are many ways that you can record from many, thousands of neurons.

[739] That's not the only way.

[740] It's a very interesting way.

[741] We and others are using brain penetrating electrodes that actually get quite deep.

[742] The whole structure of the brain is very interesting.

[743] There's the surface cortex where it's the most recently emergent part of the brain in evolution.

[744] Mammals have it.

[745] Reptiles have something a little bit like it, but it's not really the full thing.

[746] This is a very recent thing.

[747] That's what we can access with some.

[748] of these, you know, like the neuralink approach and with some of these short electrodes, this part of the brain in the cortex is only a few millimeters thick.

[749] There's so much that's deep, though, that's so important.

[750] There's the striatum, there's the thalamus.

[751] There are the parts of the brain that drive motivation that drive hunger and thirst and social interaction and parenting and flight and fear and anxiety.

[752] All these things are, there's so much that's deep that these surface approaches are not getting to.

[753] And so we and others are using these very long electrodes that help us get deep.

[754] And we can still record for many cells, many thousands of cells.

[755] We can have multiple of these at once in the same animal.

[756] And so there's a diversity of methods to get to this goal.

[757] I think it's great that people coming from, you know, outside academia will bring ideas that weren't being worked on, at least approaches.

[758] they may turn out to be synergistic.

[759] These things do work very well with optogenetics because all these electrical recording methods, that's one channel of information flow.

[760] Light delivery is a separate, you know, more or less independent.

[761] There can be some artifacts that happen, but if you're careful, it's another independent pathway of information flow.

[762] And we've done really fun experiments in mice where we play in patterns of activity with light and we record activity from all across the brain of a mouse.

[763] mouse electrically.

[764] And so using optical and electrical together is extremely powerful.

[765] So like optoelectric brain computer interfaces.

[766] Which, by the way, there's efforts on the computing side to build optoelectric servers.

[767] So like where you have both electricity.

[768] So because optics is really interesting.

[769] Light is a very interesting method of communication that's like you said, orthogonal in many ways.

[770] It doesn't have some of the constraints of bandwidth.

[771] that electricity does, going through wires, but you're able to, but less ability to control precisely at scale.

[772] So there's challenges and there's benefits, and having those two interplay is really, really, really fascinating.

[773] Especially when, obviously, on the other side of your signal is a biological mesh, mush.

[774] Mush, mushy mesh.

[775] Well, the mushy mesh is kind of interesting because there are problems with light, light scatters in the brain.

[776] So the photons don't just go, you know, linearly through.

[777] Whenever they hit an interface between fat and water, lipid and water, they bounce off in different directions.

[778] And so you can come in with all the resolution you want.

[779] You could play in an incredibly detailed high -resolution pattern of light, but the photons start scattering quite quickly, and by the time you've gone a couple of millimeters deep, you've lost almost all that fine spatial information.

[780] So, but we've developed workarounds.

[781] The longer wavelength light you use, if you get into the infrared, there's less scattering.

[782] You can use two photon methods or three photon methods where the photons have to arrive all together at the same time.

[783] You can put in fiber optics.

[784] We developed these fiber optic methods in 2007, where you can access these deep structures with fiber optic methods, and you can put many of these fiber optics at the same time in an animal.

[785] We've used holographic methods, 3D holograms to play in hundreds of individual cell -sized spots of light, and we can change those quickly.

[786] And so there are a lot of tricks, a lot of interesting optics engineering that has come together with neuroscience in a pretty exciting way.

[787] Well, that is engineering, too, which is super, super, super, super exciting.

[788] I should mention because I remember, I mentioned Elon, I recently got, for the first time ever, got COVID.

[789] How did I go so long without, finally, so I'm all vaccinated and everything like that.

[790] And so I got, because I think he mentioned it publicly so I can mention it, but I won't mention anybody else involved.

[791] But hanging out, we all got, Elon got COVID, and the interesting thing about, maybe you can comment about this.

[792] So I was only sick for like a half a day.

[793] I got a fever of like 104.

[794] I just went up and then crashed.

[795] And then I was, now, maybe I'm just seeing the silver lining of everything, but afterwards, I have like a greater clarity about the world.

[796] you just think it's greater clarity maybe maybe i just it was so uh maybe so intensely the mind fog kind of thing for such a short amount of time but the people weren't involved were also reporting this it's kind of interesting it's like uh because i do know like the immune system uh is involved with the brain in very interesting ways so like the human mind also incorporates all these other it's not just the, it's not just the nervous system.

[797] And I just wonder, because everyone always says, no, not like, everyone always says like COVID does all these bad things or whatever the disease is or whatever the virus, but I wonder like, I hate to be a Stephen Pinker on this, but like I wonder what the benefits of certain disease are if you're able to recover.

[798] Like what is there some like, again, don't want to romanticize it, but if your system goes to some kind of hardship and you come out on the other end, I wonder sometimes if there's a greater, maybe killed off a bunch of neurons that I didn't need anyway and they were actually getting in the way.

[799] There were the hater neurons.

[800] I don't know.

[801] Well, that was your inner critic that I was talking about.

[802] Exactly.

[803] You killed off your critic.

[804] Well, you know, there are mechanisms for what, the potential mechanisms for what you're talking about.

[805] There are, there's actually been a fair bit of research on post -COVID neurological function.

[806] Actually, my wife, Michelle Mongey, who's at Stanford, this work.

[807] Akiko Iwasaki, Yale has done a lot of this.

[808] But what they've found is that there's a loss of myelin.

[809] This is the coating of those long -range projections that go from one part of the brain to another.

[810] Myelin is this sort of insulator that coats these long -range projections and makes the impulses go faster and more reliably.

[811] And there's altered function of the myelin producing cells, an altered myelin in the case of COVID.

[812] They've looked in both mouse and human brains.

[813] But, of course, it could be very idiosyncratic.

[814] Many people have cognitive problems post -COVID.

[815] You're definitely aware of that.

[816] So many people report this persistent brain fog and the ability to function.

[817] But it depends on where the inflammation was.

[818] You know, maybe the people who have dysfunction post -COVID.

[819] they had a global effect.

[820] Maybe you lost some of these projections that were restraining you in some way, and these plausibly exist, and it's known that there are cell populations in the prefrontal cortex that actively restrain deeper structures from expressing what they do, and it's theoretically possible that you had a lucky, uh, focal, somebody has to get lucky, right?

[821] Somebody has to get lucky, yeah.

[822] Why not me?

[823] All right.

[824] If we can actually go back to this idea of trying through optogenetics to find origins of when the wave first starts, origins of a decision, origin of idea, origin of maybe consciousness or the subjective experience, so origin of things in the mind.

[825] So one thing, it's Carl Jung, is there a God neuron?

[826] Is there a belief neuron?

[827] Is there, so through this methodology of optogenetics, can you start getting, like where a belief begins or an idea begins?

[828] And especially looking at the strongest of our beliefs, maybe beliefs of love and hate, but religious belief into something like really grand, and the grandest of scale.

[829] Neuroscience and neurology point us a little bit.

[830] We don't have an answer to that.

[831] A lot of these questions I'm going to ask you, there's no good answer, but you're providing the tools that give us hope to find the answer one day.

[832] Yeah.

[833] And we have early clues.

[834] So for example, when patients with epilepsy have experiences of religiosity as part of their seizure or the aura before their seizure, very often those are in the temporal lobe, in these parts of the brain that are at the side.

[835] And so that's an initial clue.

[836] There are also parts of the brain that are involved in the definition of the self and defining the borders or boundaries of the self.

[837] And we know this, there's just some experiments that we did in my lab, there's a part of the brain where if there's a rhythm of a particular type, you can cause a separation of the sense of self from the sense of the body.

[838] What's normally bound up in unitary, we normally think of ourself and, we normally think of and our body is pretty tightly bound up together.

[839] Those can be separated, it turns out.

[840] We can't take that for granted, and there are certain conditions, certain patterns of activity in one part of the brain called the retrosplenial cortex, where you can actually separate those two out.

[841] And so if you think about these very big questions, you know, what is, where are the origins of religiosity?

[842] Where, how do we define the boundaries of who we are relative to others and to the world?

[843] How do we link our self to our body and how can that become separated?

[844] These are actually, believe it or not, now accessible and rigorously and quantitatively.

[845] So we did an experiment with optogenetics where we provided this abnormal rhythm to this particular part of the mouse brain.

[846] And we saw this separation of detection of a stimulus and caring about it.

[847] So that's like stimulating something about the Miles' brain that affects these neurons that give the conception of self.

[848] So you're able to dissociate the experience from the impact of the experience unto you.

[849] That's right, exactly right.

[850] So like these are the goals of meditation.

[851] These are the goals whenever I get drunk, pretty much effective.

[852] I mean, that's not a scientific statement, just an experiential anecdotal one.

[853] also psychedelics seek to this to attain this kind of state that's so interesting well you mentioned psychedelics you know DMT and 5MEO DMT these these create this religious experience this connection people describe them as a strong connection to God that in theory these are accessible with with modern methods we we now that we have these rich recording methods we can explore what are that precise millisecond resolution, cellular resolution, brainwide manifestations of these altered states.

[854] So, like, you could look at an altered state, like on DMT, recorded across many people, and then from there, see where do these experiences originate in the brain in terms of single neurons, and then how do they propagate and interact with everything else?

[855] and if there's some kind of common signal.

[856] Like, how do you narrow down the set of neurons that are responsible for a particular experience or for a particular behavioral effect?

[857] Here's where optogenetics is so useful because any time you give an agent, like, you know, an agent like ketamine or PCP, which we used for our dissociation experiments that I was mentioning, or you have a psychedelic LSD or DMT for this altered, perceptual state.

[858] If you give either of those, these change everything across the brain.

[859] Okay, so just the fact that you maybe give them to a mouse, let's say, or eventually to a human, you won't know yet which cells to home in on as the causal players in all this, just by recording the activity.

[860] But then what we found is that optogenetics providing a causal pattern of activity guided by what you see can let you test hypotheses.

[861] And we saw this rhythm with ketamine and PCP for dissociation.

[862] And then we said, okay, let's test what's causal.

[863] We came in and provided that rhythm.

[864] We tried a few different things, but only one of the causal tests we tried actually caused the behavioral dissociation.

[865] And so that's how we home in on what actually matters.

[866] And is it repeatable once you see the causal.

[867] So like that's one definition of causality is like you try and it repeats.

[868] across different mice and all that kind of stuff.

[869] And so you could do that for DMT.

[870] You could do that for the really fascinating mind -expanding drug.

[871] Thank you.

[872] Thank you for the meme for people just listening.

[873] This is, again, another disagreement between Freud and Carl Jung.

[874] Religion and spirituality.

[875] This is, I guess, the ring scene from Lord of the Rings.

[876] Religion and spirituality, Freud says, cast it into the fire, destroy it.

[877] Carl Jung says, no. So for people who don't know, Sergei is the Slavic Lord of the Meme, thank you, I appreciate that.

[878] So what we're talking about, so do there is, I mean, I think a connection between DMT and religious experiences or some of these psychedelics.

[879] Do you think it's possible to sort of stimulate religious experiences?

[880] So religious experiences are one of the most deep kind of experiences.

[881] And so here you could, first understand where they originate, how they propagate to the brain, and then to stimulate them.

[882] Yeah.

[883] And so this is, and these that can happen in people who had no predisposition, you know, people who are, you know, as agnostic or atheistic as you'd like, they can have these, they can feel connected to God in these states.

[884] Now, to be clear, I'm not advocating these.

[885] We don't know what's safe in human beings.

[886] But we definitely can do these experiments in mice, and that was already very productive in understanding dissociation.

[887] So we can already imagine making headway on these methods.

[888] And then, you know, I had a, and this does map on to the non -psychedelic human experience.

[889] I had a patient who's actually described in the book, projections.

[890] This was the patient that's in the mania chapter, the bipolar chapter.

[891] here was a guy who had never had a psychiatric illness or symptom in his life he was a retirement age gentleman and nobody in his family either so no family history no personal history of any psychiatric illness and he had never been religious particularly before either certainly no no passionate you know type of religion but he not through any psychedelic or drug he he had a stressful experience actually a post 9 -11 change in how he was thinking, and he was pushed into a mania, a manic state, revealing that he had bipolar, never before known in this case, in this person.

[892] And his mania, his elevated state and bipolar included this profound religiosity, which he had never had before.

[893] And he was, he was, you know, preaching in a elevated, you know, vigorous way to his family.

[894] And so this, this state can be.

[895] created in people even late in life who had no predisposition for it and no even without a neurochemical.

[896] So there's the causality of that is very interesting to explore.

[897] How did the manic state unleash this religiosity?

[898] But you see that in other realms of psychiatry too, OCD can manifest as religiosity also.

[899] You can take people who never really had a religion never played a powerful role in their life.

[900] But then when their obsessive -compulsive symptoms become severe, they can manifest in this.

[901] I think I'm in that group.

[902] So I'm a bit OCD.

[903] We have, there's, I think these is sub -reddits when there's oddly satisfying things.

[904] So there's certain things that are really satisfying to my OCD, and my mild OCD.

[905] I think it's pretty much a religious experience.

[906] So I understand that there's, if it's not direct, it's at least rhymes.

[907] So maybe can you speak to the, as Sergei is probably desperately scrambling to pull up oddly satisfying.

[908] Thank you.

[909] People can check it out themselves.

[910] It is as the sub -breddit promises, oddly satisfying.

[911] Can we talk about bipolar?

[912] Maybe depression.

[913] Well, let's talk about, I mean, I don't know if there's a nice way to discuss the differences in the full landscape of suffering.

[914] that's here, but maybe what is depression and what are the types of depression?

[915] What kind of depression have you seen and experienced and researched and how can people overcome it?

[916] How can humans overcome it and deal with it, live with it, overcome it?

[917] So this is my clinical specialty.

[918] I see patients in my outpatient clinical work with treatment -resistant depression, so very hard -to -treat severe illness where medications haven't been working.

[919] I also see patients with autism spectrum disorders.

[920] These are my two clinical focal areas.

[921] But then I do emergency room work as well.

[922] But the depression, why do I focus on that?

[923] It's so one feels tantalizingly close to helping these people who are, suffering so deeply.

[924] And that's why I focused on it is these are people who, there may not even be anything situational that's difficult or challenging in their life.

[925] You can have people who seem to have everything that you would want.

[926] Every objective measure of the life is fine.

[927] And yet they can be just hit with this unstoppable hopelessness and inability to see into the future.

[928] a discounting of the value of their own action.

[929] Anything they can imagine themselves doing seems worthless or they are unable to enjoy things.

[930] We call this Anhedonia.

[931] There's no reward, no pleasure, not in food, social interaction, movies, books, anything that they would enjoy.

[932] Positivity gone.

[933] They can have a profound negative internal state, psychic pain.

[934] and these things can seem, and in the severe cases, are inescapable.

[935] So what is going on?

[936] Why is this state part of human existence?

[937] It's got a strong biological genetic link.

[938] We know that.

[939] It's been linked to certain genes, certain regions of the chromosomes, and twin studies.

[940] There's a clear genetic link.

[941] It doesn't explain everything, but it's a big part of it.

[942] genetics are a strong contributor and although you can have depression without anything terrible going on in your life the symptoms can be made worse by stressors by trauma and but at a very deep level there's nothing we can measure in a person objectively so we don't have there's not a known chemical not a known structure that's different not a known brain activity pattern that we can pick up with EEG.

[943] A lot of people are exploring this, but right now we have no objective measures.

[944] All we do is talk to people, and we elicit these symptoms.

[945] We explore them, distinguish them from other possible causes, and then what do we do?

[946] Well, we have a range of treatments.

[947] We have medications that can help people, do help people, but not everybody.

[948] And if they don't work, then we can go to brain stimulation methods.

[949] We can do things even like electroconvulsions, therapy, which is very effective, but it's sort of the final thing we go to in the end.

[950] And so we have treatments.

[951] They work for some people.

[952] They don't do everything we'd like.

[953] But here's the problem is at a very deep level, we don't understand really what's going on in the brain.

[954] We don't have a physical interpretation of the problem.

[955] We have all these symptoms, but we can't yet point to a set of cells or a set of circuits or an activity pattern that is causing major depression, this disease state per se in human beings.

[956] Why do you think you can't yet from an auto -genetics perspective?

[957] Is it because there's so many possible causes?

[958] Is it so much, so many things involved?

[959] So I think the answer is there are many things involved and all these different symptoms that I've mentioned.

[960] Those we can study and those we can fix, the individual symptoms.

[961] And we can do this in animals, to be clear.

[962] so in a mouse, for example, we can instantaneously and precisely turn up or down the motivation of an animal to overcome a challenge.

[963] We can turn up or down its ability to be motivated by or we think experience reward from situations or actions.

[964] We can increase its apparent energy level, its drive to meet challenges.

[965] We can turn up or down, social interaction, all these individual features of depression, individual symptoms we now can point to exact projections and cells that are causal in mediating these.

[966] But we don't know is why all these different symptoms show up together in major depression, in the human disease syndrome.

[967] And that's the mystery.

[968] It's sort of in other fields of medicine, you know, someone with congestive heart failure who comes into the clinic, they have very different symptoms.

[969] They have shortness of breath and they have swollen feet.

[970] Okay.

[971] Couldn't be two more different across the body sets of symptoms.

[972] Neither one obviously related to the heart, but they're both happening because the heart is not working as a pump.

[973] Okay.

[974] And now, thankfully in cardiology, we understand these disparate symptoms that seem to be.

[975] totally unrelated can be completely understood because there's an altered pump action of the heart that's that's what we are hoping for in in psychiatry and in in the study of depression or any disease these different symptoms the inability to enjoy things the hopelessness uh what's the what's the unifying unifying i mean is there is there some truth to that the tolstoy quote that all happy families are alike and each unhappy family is unhappy in its own way.

[976] So basically, I mean, this is the human condition.

[977] And basically, that might, you know, the physicists long to find the theory of everything.

[978] It isn't understanding depression essentially require you to really have the big theory of everything for the human mind.

[979] I think we, it would certainly be nice to have that a theory of everything.

[980] Don't get me wrong.

[981] I don't think we need a understatement of the century.

[982] It would be nice.

[983] It's also a good question if it's possible.

[984] Yeah.

[985] Yeah.

[986] Well, that I have some thoughts on to.

[987] But to this specific question, I don't think we need a theory of everything.

[988] I think there will be unifying principles we can get to.

[989] But even shy of that, if we can treat symptoms, and that's a big step.

[990] And as you say, different different unhappy families are different, different unhappy people are different.

[991] If we have somebody who comes to the clinic and I see someone with a profound anhedonia as one of their main symptoms, inability to enjoy things, and if I know, based on optogenetics work and animal work, that a particular medication can treat anahedonia, even if it doesn't fix major depression in everybody, if I treat that one symptom and that one person, that's a good thing.

[992] And so we don't need the theory of everything, and we don't even need the unifying principle to help people with insights that come from optogenetics.

[993] How much does talking help for diagnosis and for treatment, would you say, for depression?

[994] It's a big part of what we do.

[995] Every good psychiatrist should be a pretty adept in these verbal communications and talk therapy as part of what they do.

[996] I give medications.

[997] I deliver brain stimulation treatments, but a big, big part of everything I do with every patient is talk therapy, because it works so well together with these other modalities.

[998] Even alone, it can help people with moderate or mild depression by itself.

[999] People with severe depression, people with other psychiatric illnesses that are severe, you don't want to do talk therapy alone.

[1000] It's not going to do it, but it still is crucial.

[1001] to do together with the others.

[1002] And it's critical because it's part of how you reshape cognitions, you know, complex activity patterns, and you won't get to that with a medication or brain stimulation treatment.

[1003] Do you have advice for people who suffer from mild forms of depression or feel as they might both for those people, and do you have advice for people who love the people who suffer from depression?

[1004] and want to help.

[1005] One of the incredibly frustrating things about depression is the very nature of it makes it hard for the people who suffer to get treatment because they're hopeless so they don't think treatment will help.

[1006] They have low energy so they're not motivated to participate in treatment in many cases.

[1007] Sometimes they're actively suicidal.

[1008] That certainly doesn't help.

[1009] they have all these things that seem to prevent treatment from being effective.

[1010] So the loved ones, that's where the loved ones are so important, is helping them overcome these barriers to treatment, the motivation, the safety, and the insight.

[1011] That's critical, and particularly for the severe cases.

[1012] For the mild cases, where people still have some insight and motivation and energy to get something done, there are many things you can do exercise is extremely important in mood maintenance regulation of sleep and getting sufficient and regular enough sleep is very important and talk therapy can be helpful in those mild or moderate cases just looking at cognitions looking at patterns of thought that people may have fallen into where they catastrophize where they spiral from small things into big things.

[1013] A little bit of talk therapy, 10, 12 sessions can help people identify those patterns they may have in themselves that are taking occasional negative thoughts, which everybody has, and magnifying those into more persistent negative states.

[1014] Once you, if you work at this, it's kind of like homework.

[1015] This is what we call cognitive behavioral therapy.

[1016] It's very structured, very organized.

[1017] You work hard.

[1018] It requires insight and motivation, and you have to be motivated.

[1019] But if you are, then you can identify these triggers that send you down particular pathways and work to intercept them.

[1020] And that is amazingly very effective in mild to moderate cases.

[1021] So you basically have to train yourself to see the world as a collection of triggers.

[1022] And you have to first understand, like collect the data, like basically see every experience.

[1023] experience as a thing that creates a follow -on emotion of feeling.

[1024] And like, I've learned this, you know, like on social media, where, like, early on, you know, like all of us, you know, I'll say something, I kind of respond to negativity with negativity, and then you observe the result of that.

[1025] Over time, you think, wait a minute, this thing that I've been doing where when somebody says, you suck and you say, no, you suck, that never produces the result you thought it might.

[1026] And so I might not want to just don't say you suck back.

[1027] And I do this through a lot of things in life.

[1028] I'm very fortunate to not suffer from depression but I, first of all, I have had and have people in my life who do.

[1029] And also, you know, all of us have depression who don't suffer from depression, have depression out.

[1030] Like, it's always knocking on the door.

[1031] Right.

[1032] And so you have mild, I mean, if you're very careless with the triggers all around you, then you, you're just, I think all of us have the capacity to really suffer from that kind of chemical or psychological or philosophical existential crisis.

[1033] But then it raises a question, why are we built this way?

[1034] It seems like it doesn't make sense, right?

[1035] And here's where some of us thinking about where we came from as the human family is kind of It doesn't make sense that somewhere on that spectrum, that it's good to detect that there's an array of adverse forces out there in the world right now at this moment, and to withdraw, to hunker down, to not fight, not strive, not try to meet the challenge, and outweigh these negative forces that are present out there.

[1036] And that makes a lot of sense.

[1037] And all animals that have been studied in one form or another show this.

[1038] Even the worm that I mentioned earlier, Cialigans with 302 neurons.

[1039] It can effectively give up in challenging situations.

[1040] We've done this with zebrafish, tiny little transparent fish.

[1041] You can give them a challenging situation and they will give up.

[1042] But then if you stimulate a couple very specific brain regions in particular ways, you can motivate them to overcome the challenge.

[1043] And if you inhibit those regions, they give up much more easily than they would otherwise.

[1044] You can do this in mice, you can do this in rats.

[1045] So this is an ancestral conserve pattern to detect that things are pretty bad out there and to conserve energy, to hunker down, to wait out the storm.

[1046] So as use, unfortunately, many of our maladies have useful roots in our, that contribute to our survival.

[1047] So both depression and motivation have uses.

[1048] And sometimes it's nice to just shut the hell up and huddle with the penguins versus, for some unknown reason, venture out on your own into the mountains, like a David Goggins type character.

[1049] So what's the difference to you between, you see patience, between sort of rigorous, psychoanalysis.

[1050] I don't know if you consider what you, like, talk therapy and psychoanalysis, are they neighbors?

[1051] Are they overlapping?

[1052] They're neighbors.

[1053] Psychoanalysis is, it's a, they're relatively, it's not nearly done as much as the talk therapy, like the cognitive behavioral therapy I mentioned.

[1054] The psychoanalysis is, is a little more niche now, and partly because it's not, data isn't, in terms of actual treatment of actual therapeutic effects, data not as supportive as for cognitive behavioral therapy.

[1055] But it's still interesting as for insight.

[1056] People, a lot of people still do it to gain insight into themselves.

[1057] And in general, it's a good sort of conversation starter.

[1058] Those methods, you know, they're good for getting things out.

[1059] You know, we don't focus on dreams typically these days in psychiatry, but they're great conversation starters.

[1060] They're great ways to get things out if people have.

[1061] And so we like to use those methods just to get the ball rolling sometimes, get people to open up a little bit.

[1062] But the actual treatment tends not to involve these psychoanalytic approaches where you are really probing the unconscious mind and its manifestation through dreams, for example, as the goal.

[1063] That's not the goal.

[1064] Modern talk therapy, we're really focusing on treatment, how to get people to feel better.

[1065] Yeah, I use that as a conversation opener, the Freudian thing, where I try to delve at a bar of the deep sexual desires in a person's subconscious, and I find that opens up possibilities very quickly.

[1066] No, I mean, this is a silly sounding question, but what's the difference between cognitive behavioral therapy and conversation?

[1067] So, because I personally, as a fan of conversations, is a fan of just I like listening to podcasts versus like audiobook.

[1068] I like both, but they're very different.

[1069] And I like conversation.

[1070] I like, it makes me personally very anxious, so I like to be the listener, like a third wheel, like overhearing a conversation kind of thing.

[1071] But it's a really powerful method for humans to explore each other's mind, just raw conversation.

[1072] So do you think it can be more productive to be very systematic about it or is conversation itself the art form of helping each other, understanding each other and helping each other?

[1073] There are forms of talk therapy that are essentially conversational, or they much more approach peer conversation.

[1074] There's a befriending therapy.

[1075] There's interpersonal therapy.

[1076] These are approaches that are purely talk therapy, but they're not as structured as cognitive behavioral therapy.

[1077] Cognitive behavioral therapy, as there are manuals, there are guidelines.

[1078] You can almost go through it in a very cookbooky way.

[1079] There's homework that you get done.

[1080] So it's, in its fullest form, it's very different from these more conversational strategies.

[1081] But what's interesting is sometimes people compare them, and so you'll see almost like randomized controlled studies comparing cognitive behavioral therapy with interpersonal therapy, for example.

[1082] And they both can work.

[1083] And actually, in some studies, they look comparable.

[1084] So there's, to your point, conversation and insights that come from conversation, if done well, if done artfully, can be as powerful.

[1085] This reminds me of Robin Williams.

[1086] I have to ask you several questions here on that.

[1087] But one of my favorite movies is Goodwill Hunting.

[1088] I don't know if you've seen it with Robin Williams.

[1089] So as a psychiatrist yourself, can you do a deep analysis of this other famous psychiatrist, which is the movie character, played by Robin Williams at Goodwill Hunting.

[1090] Is it just a caricature between a psychiatrist and patient relationship, or is there something to you that was moving about his ability to connect to this obviously struggling young kid?

[1091] I think you've hit on the key thing there, which is the depth of the connection.

[1092] If there's too powerful connection, that can impair therapy because it could impair open communication.

[1093] If someone, if a patient has a, sees the role, sees the relationship in a particular way, like in a friendly way, maybe, or like a parental child type way, that can cause problems because then what they choose to share, what they choose to bring up is selected to be appropriate for that.

[1094] view of the relationship.

[1095] And so I and many other talk therapists actually prefer not to let things get, not let the connection get that deep.

[1096] You want to have trust, you want to have a therapeutic alliance, we sometimes call it, but it's got to be enough of a blank slate that the patient is not consciously or unconsciously constrained in what they, choose to share.

[1097] And so great movie, great actors, all good, no complaints except realistically the relationship should be a little more arm's length than that.

[1098] Let's pretend this is real life.

[1099] Sometimes can't you leave a little bit of yourself in the interaction with the patient?

[1100] I mean, it's another human being.

[1101] Yes.

[1102] So it's a balance and actually you do need some of it, because let's say this person is having challenges, interpersonal challenges in their life.

[1103] The best way to notice what those are and to identify them and to work with them is if you can elicit some of those problems in the office, in the therapeutic interaction.

[1104] And this is really powerful.

[1105] As long as you're alert to it, aware of it, and you don't let it go out of hand, this transference, we call it, is when you transfer in between the current therapeutic relationship and external relationships that the patient may have had with others.

[1106] And so if the therapist starts to feel an inner feeling like anger, let's say.

[1107] So let's say you have a patient who is stirring frustration in you, or even in extreme case is anger.

[1108] The best thing for the therapist to do in that case is to recognize it and to realize that's probably being stirred by other people in the patient's life, and that could be the source of a lot of problems.

[1109] And so instead of trying to wall it off and say, oh, I shouldn't be feeling that.

[1110] I better be a better therapist instead and recognize it and use it and help the patient that way.

[1111] And so you've got to be a human being.

[1112] You've got to be a person who feels.

[1113] and you've got to be open.

[1114] But be in control of it and be aware of it.

[1115] If I may, I just want to read the, because it's my favorite scenes.

[1116] Probably one of the greatest scenes, one of the greatest scenes in movie history because Robin Williams does a single take.

[1117] Is that right?

[1118] I didn't know.

[1119] So this is a very interesting interaction between them.

[1120] So, Will, and I'm sure this is a common interaction, maybe with a third person of a patient, maybe with a father and son, where Will, the young character, they're a young brilliant mathematician, and Sean is the therapist, the older therapist, where Will looks at a painting that Sean painted and then does a deep critical analysis of the painting that basically, you know, describes pretending as if he can understand another human being completely but just looking at their painting.

[1121] And then Sean gives this whole speech that contrasts, sort of raw intelligence and the wisdom of experience.

[1122] And Sean says, single take.

[1123] He says, you've never been out of Boston, right?

[1124] And Will says, nope.

[1125] All this in a sexy Boston accent, by the way.

[1126] And then Sean gives this speech.

[1127] If I asked you about art, you'd probably give me this skinny and about every art book ever written.

[1128] Michelangelo, you know a lot about him.

[1129] Life's work, political aspirations, him and the Pope, sexual orientation.

[1130] The whole works, right?

[1131] right?

[1132] But I bet you can't tell me what it smells like in the Sistine Chapel.

[1133] You've never actually stood there and looked up at that beautiful ceiling, seen that.

[1134] If I asked you about women, you'll probably give me a syllabus of your personal favorites.

[1135] You may have even been laid a few times.

[1136] The language here is just beautiful.

[1137] But you can't tell me what it feels like to wake up to a woman and feel truly happy.

[1138] You're a tough kid.

[1139] If I asked you about war, you'd probably throw Shakespeare at me, right?

[1140] Probably not, but let's say.

[1141] Once more into the breach of your friends, but you've never been near one.

[1142] You've never held your best friend's head on your lap and watch him gasp his last breath, looking to you for help.

[1143] If I ask you about love, you'd probably quote me a sonnet.

[1144] But you've never looked at a woman and be truly vulnerable.

[1145] Known someone who can level you with their eyes, feeling like God put an angel on earth just for you, who could rescue you from the depths of hell and you wouldn't know what it's like to be her angel.

[1146] To have that love for her, be there forever, through anything, through cancer.

[1147] And you wouldn't know about sleeping, sitting up in a hospital room for two months, holding her hand because the doctors could see in your eyes, the terms visiting hours don't apply to you.

[1148] You don't know about real loss, because that only occurs when you love something more than you love yourself.

[1149] I doubt you've ever dared to love anybody that much.

[1150] I look at you.

[1151] I don't see an intelligent, confident man. I see a cocky, scared, shitless kid.

[1152] But you're a genius, Will.

[1153] No one denies that.

[1154] No one can possibly understand the depths of you, but you presumed to know everything about me because you saw a painting of mine, you ripped my fucking life apart.

[1155] You're an orphan, right?

[1156] Do you think I know the first thing about how hard your life has been, how you feel who you are because I read Oliver Twist?

[1157] Does that encapsulate you?

[1158] Personally, I don't give a shit about all that, because you know what?

[1159] I can't learn anything from you that I can't read in some fucking book unless you want to talk about you.

[1160] who you are, and I'm fascinated.

[1161] Amen.

[1162] But you don't want to do that, do you sport?

[1163] You're terrified of what you might say.

[1164] You're a move, Chief.

[1165] Well done, sir.

[1166] I know it's a movie.

[1167] It's interesting, right?

[1168] So some of that conversation is at some intellectual level, too.

[1169] It's not just emotional.

[1170] It's something, it's like, the reason I kind of connect with that is that's a lot of work for a therapist.

[1171] Like, to really understand another, because he's, I mean, from, okay, I know this is fictional, but just there's calculation happening.

[1172] He deeply cares to say the words that the other person needs to hear, but also a little bit loses himself in the pride, but then catches himself again, switches from anger to connection.

[1173] Yeah.

[1174] A lot is brought up there.

[1175] You're right.

[1176] there has to be some emotion in the therapist to care enough to keep going to to keep probing to open up as he's doing so right he revealed a lot about himself his own vulnerabilities but that gave him authenticity he had to open himself up so that the the kid would see the authenticity and open himself up in return so so how do you do that as a as a psychiatrist as a therapist.

[1177] You have to be careful.

[1178] You know, you don't want to do too much, uh, but opening up a little bit does help.

[1179] It does create, uh, a chance.

[1180] You know, you're offering up something and that helps the patient come back in return.

[1181] And it gives, it gives you that, that, uh, believability and authenticity.

[1182] Do you pay the price for that for opening it up?

[1183] You can.

[1184] Um, you have a family, you have an incredibly like difficult research like you're doing a lot of things in your world I mean it's a price you pay for like well this is one of the terrifying things about about writing the book was I do open up in a little bit about my own personal life my own personal challenges and that was a considered decision because I could I could have done the patient work and the science work and the and the history of the human family work and tied it all together but but it wasn't in an early draft it was it was like that but it wasn't it wasn't real yet it wasn't it wasn't something that that everybody could connect with and I said then I realized look if I'm going to do this I've got a I've got to open up myself and then and then people can connect with me and and see what I'm really saying.

[1185] And so I did.

[1186] And that was, it was not something that I'd gone in planning to do.

[1187] In retrospect, I learned a lot about myself.

[1188] It was actually really, I think, a good thing that I did, but it was scary.

[1189] Were the darkest places you've ever gone in your life?

[1190] You know, I had, things haven't always been easy personally or, you know, or professionally.

[1191] I had moments, you know, I was effectively a single dad for, for a while, a number of years, and these came at probably the hardest also professional lifetimes for me, too.

[1192] The absolute hardest days of late medical school, internship, you know, taking call, you know, getting up at 3 a .m., you know, surgery, medicine, rounds, unforgiving environments, and then all the while, you know, personal life, you know, stripped down to the bear.

[1193] And these were low moments, and then I was hit particularly hard by just experiences on the clinical ward connecting to deeply with patients like a child with a brain tumor and feeling it too strongly.

[1194] And those things, when you get down to those lowest of the low moments when everything is stripped away and there's only this raw core, well, yeah, that's pretty hard.

[1195] That was probably the lowest moment.

[1196] And you learn a lot about yourself in those moments.

[1197] You know, what's left and then what are the roots out from there?

[1198] And that can be powerful to see in yourself.

[1199] Have you thought about killing yourself?

[1200] I have not.

[1201] I have not.

[1202] Have you seen that thought in the distance?

[1203] I am fortunate that that has not come to my mind.

[1204] and I have not seen it even in the distance.

[1205] And in some ways, I've wondered if that's made me, am I a less effective psychiatrist because of that?

[1206] I've felt everything stripped away.

[1207] I've been at the lowest of the low, and yet that...

[1208] There's a light of hope, there's a light of hope still at the end of the tunnel.

[1209] So you've never lost even for brief moments that...

[1210] Never did.

[1211] I don't know why.

[1212] You don't know why.

[1213] There was no reason.

[1214] You don't know why.

[1215] No reason to feel hope at that moment, honestly.

[1216] So it was just a light without reason.

[1217] Yeah, that's right.

[1218] What wisdom do you draw from that time about...

[1219] So first of all, you said something funny, which is I wonder if it...

[1220] That it's somehow not having thoughts of suicide limits your capacity to truly understand on somebody who is having those thoughts.

[1221] So how many demons must the psychiatrist have in order to be a good psychiatrist?

[1222] Yeah, you know, this is a really interesting question.

[1223] I think everybody knows, and I can say this, that psychiatrists can be a little unusual.

[1224] We think about ourselves, right?

[1225] We think about our brains.

[1226] That may be one reason why we become psychiatrists.

[1227] We think, oh, that's interesting going on in there.

[1228] What's that about?

[1229] So a little introspective, a little introverted maybe, and that's what can make us good when we're good.

[1230] But also that may select for people who have some unusual aspects, but you don't have to have all of them.

[1231] There's a lot that can go wrong in the psychiatric realm.

[1232] I think having some of it, but not all of it, is enough.

[1233] You get to see how low things can get.

[1234] you can get a, you get empathy from that, even if the symptoms are not the same.

[1235] Just empathy for struggle, for suffering.

[1236] That's right.

[1237] That's right.

[1238] Do you yourself have to practice observing triggers just as a human operating in this world?

[1239] I've definitely, those skills that have come from therapy, I've found them useful.

[1240] Yeah.

[1241] If I noticed that we've all been, through experiences where we wonder, oh, I got really mad in that interaction.

[1242] Why did I get that mad?

[1243] Yeah, sure, maybe I could have been irritated, but man, why did I?

[1244] And then thinking about it and realizing, okay, you know, back up here, think about the broader context, think about how that relates to, you know, prior events in my life.

[1245] Okay, yeah, so this is a thing for me when something of this class happens, then it triggers me. So going forward, I'm going to be aware of that.

[1246] And I've definitely used that because it's, you don't want to be out of control of those emotions.

[1247] You want to identify them.

[1248] You want to know where they come from.

[1249] And you want to head them off as a civilized human being living on this earth, trying to get along with other people.

[1250] You want to understand those moments.

[1251] Let me return to Robin Williams for a second and looking at Robin Williams, the the actor, the, sorry, the human.

[1252] And because you mentioned for depression, you can have everything going well.

[1253] And I think there's just famous cases of just public figures, because a lot of people know them, where they suffer quietly, and it seems like from the outside perspective that they have everything going for them, that they're at top of their career.

[1254] you know, two people that come to mind are Robin Williams and Anthony Bourdain.

[1255] What insight do you have and why either of those have taken, why Robin Williams, a comedian, one of sort of the most jolly humans?

[1256] Obviously, there was always the darkness that he was channeling in order to present the happiness.

[1257] But it feels like that realness is only possible when you're deeply self -honest and analytical.

[1258] And then if you're deeply self -honest, you're going to realize that there's a lot of beautiful things about life that you can discover.

[1259] And if you do that, how can you possibly then take your own life?

[1260] And you go through all of these thoughts.

[1261] And I think a lot of, a lot, a lot of people really loved Robin Williams, which is why it was really difficult to see.

[1262] How can even him, how can even Robin Williams take his own life?

[1263] So I don't know if there's something to be said about the nature of depression from just looking at his case.

[1264] I think the action of suicide is not well understood.

[1265] It doesn't always, although often is correlated with depression.

[1266] There are cases of suicide where there is not clear depression that's in the minority.

[1267] By the way, if I just, because you said it's so interesting, action of suicide, because there's also thoughts of suicide.

[1268] and probably those, they're probably somewhat understood, but it's an interesting, because you can think of suicide, if you have suicidal ideation, you can think of that for so many reasons.

[1269] That's right.

[1270] And the, I mean, thoughts, sometimes, like painful thoughts, angry thoughts or thoughts in general can be very different like fantasies for example you can fantasize like sexual fantasies you can fantasize I was just for humor's sake wanted to mention stuff but then people think I'm serious I'm not going to mention anything but sexual fantasies and then there's I know there's people that have sexual fantasies and they don't want to actually do that in real life that sexual fantasy serves some kind of purpose in imagination only and that same way suicide might serve a purpose and imagination only is very unlikely to lead to action.

[1271] And yet there's other thoughts that maybe are more amorphous that do lead to action.

[1272] And that leap, yeah, that, oh, boy, that's a fascinating.

[1273] And that's such a philosophically powerful thought to not exist.

[1274] Like that question, that's this, is it Sarcher or Camus?

[1275] Camus, or the myth of Cimu, who says, like, basic question of why live?

[1276] good question yeah right so that's a great question actually and there are other related questions some people uh may have the thought of suicide uh because there seems no point there's no joy in life that's one reason that some people can put forward sometimes there's an it's not just the absence of of joy there's an active pain an active psychic pain in some people and that, the inescapability of that is enough to drive the thoughts of suicide.

[1277] And then there are interpersonal and cultural reasons as well that can show up.

[1278] But the act, this act of ending of the self is, in all these cases, there's no real way to study this in animals, no other animal as far as we know that we can study, has this concept of, this is myself, the situation is not tolerable, therefore I will end the self.

[1279] To our knowledge, this is not something that can be studied another animal.

[1280] So it remains this very poorly understood action.

[1281] And in predicting it, so what do we do as psychiatrists?

[1282] We have this challenge.

[1283] People come to the emergency room.

[1284] They say they're suicidal, or their friends say they're suicidal, or they've taken some action that didn't lead to death.

[1285] What do we do?

[1286] Well, there's a whole range of options, you know, was it a suicidal gesture in the sense of not intending death?

[1287] Or was it, was the intent death?

[1288] And if it was the intent was death, what were the reasons?

[1289] Are the reasons transient?

[1290] Are they gone now?

[1291] What's the probability that it'll be repeated?

[1292] So we do all these things just to decide what sort of treatment should be carried out.

[1293] But nowhere is there a deep understanding of the biology, of the cells and circuits.

[1294] and activity patterns that underlie the action to end the self.

[1295] It's a very, it's so, it's this frustrating thing.

[1296] It's so timely, it's so common, it shows up in veterans, it shows up in kids, it shows up in people at every stage of life, and yet we're very bad at understanding it, and we're relatively poor at predicting it, and our tools are not very powerful.

[1297] We can put people in a locked unit.

[1298] We can give them care therapy for a while.

[1299] At some point, we release them, and there's only so much we can do.

[1300] It's one of the most frustrating things, the suffering that is linked to suicidality.

[1301] But it is a decision, and it is an action, and if you look at optogenetics, you should be able to one day sort of understand the dynamics of such weighty decisions.

[1302] The individual causes, then, if someone is an hedonic, if there is no joy in life, That very likely is addressable by optogenetics.

[1303] We know how to turn that dial very robustly in animals.

[1304] The motivation to overcome challenges that we have some hope of understanding.

[1305] Psychic pain, internal negative states, we have actually a handle on that as well.

[1306] There's a structure in the brain called the habenula and some linked structures around it that seems to generate this negative internal state.

[1307] It's active when a state of acute disappointment, acute outcomes that go wrong, not as expected, you know, moments of unexpected pain.

[1308] Habenula is there, it seems, it's active to report on internal negativity with its action.

[1309] And so you could imagine strategies to target this brain structure that might have the effect of reducing psychic pain, and reducing the negativity of internal states.

[1310] That is a very concrete hope.

[1311] It's precise, it's anatomical.

[1312] Optogenetics has given us all the firm foundation we need to go after that question.

[1313] So I think there is hope.

[1314] If you look at the individual causes, the individual symptoms relating to suicide, and then it's like a puzzle, you put together the puzzle pieces.

[1315] By the way, I do think my habanula is very functioning very actively.

[1316] And I wonder if it's like, because you can also learn to channel these things, right?

[1317] Some of the things we suffer from, I mean, there's degrees of suffering, can be a source of progress and personal growth and development and all those kinds of things.

[1318] I mean, I, what is it, Nietzsche suffered from stomach issues.

[1319] you know, I wonder if he's written some of those things if his stomach was all great.

[1320] Right.

[1321] I mean, there's, I kind of think that a difficult life in some form, you can get, you get to choose in some regard and some of you don't, the difficulties you have and the ones you do have, it's nice to use if possible.

[1322] Sometimes it's nice to treat.

[1323] Sometimes it's nice to, it's just nice to use.

[1324] Well, the way you phrase it, I think you're using it.

[1325] I could be wrong.

[1326] But if you phrased in this semi -humorous way about your Hibnula, it seems to me that you're using that to good effect.

[1327] Now, but one never really knows what someone else's internal state is.

[1328] As I look at you, I don't know the depths of what's going on, and it's possible that it's a much harder situation in there.

[1329] Yes.

[1330] So I actually worry about this a lot, so I'm extremely self -critical.

[1331] Like, in the privacy of my own mind, which is an interesting thing when you get to meet the internet and the internet will tell you you suck.

[1332] But for now, this is what I worry about, and I'm very paying attention.

[1333] For now it's really, I just have this like very negative voice.

[1334] But that voice seems to be very useful for productivity.

[1335] And so I channel it.

[1336] I just put it on the table and let that voice talk to me. But I'm very, I'm like monitoring that voice.

[1337] because looking at Robin Williams, you know, you get older, your brain changes or like your, and then that voice can now all of a sudden grow, right?

[1338] And then where you can't control as much, you'd be very careful with these kinds of things.

[1339] You're very right about that.

[1340] So my negativity, I have this, I never think I've done enough is sort of where my negativity comes from inside.

[1341] I never think that I've met the potential of the moment, I haven't done, I haven't, you know, made the most of the opportunities that are available.

[1342] Still early, I haven't, you know, progressed as far as I should.

[1343] And exactly as you're saying, that works for a while.

[1344] But then what happens as you get later in life and there's less runway to, you know, fix that?

[1345] And then maybe then that negative voice is a problem.

[1346] But also at that point, the negative voice starts having more and more of a point.

[1347] When you're being very successful, it's easy to be like, no, okay, well, like, because later in life, you're really literally just sitting there on a rocking chair doing nothing.

[1348] And then it's, or maybe any kind of tragedy happens, loss of a loved one, loss of a job, loss, or you get screwed over in some kind of way.

[1349] I don't know.

[1350] And then all of a sudden, the negative voice is just you and the negative voice for days and days and days.

[1351] And so I don't know, to go back to your example of Robin Williams, I don't know what was going on inside him.

[1352] I don't know the nature of his internal state.

[1353] Was it an active psychic pain that?

[1354] May I mention, may I interrupt, to just say that Sergey posted an examination of Robin Williams' brain tissue suggested that he suffered from, quote, diffuse L -E -W -Y, Lewy body dementia, LBD.

[1355] Depression is a symptom of LBD, and it's not about psychology.

[1356] It's rooted in urology.

[1357] This is words from his brain was falling apart.

[1358] Louie body dementia, this is a very interesting neurological disorder where, among other things, there's neuron death, indeed.

[1359] So you've got frank neuron loss.

[1360] It's not just a matter of some longstanding psychic pain, but you've got a progressive, loss.

[1361] And so clearly you've got a situation where he could have finally reached a point where the balance that he'd worked out between negativity and positivity was disrupted due to loss.

[1362] The wrong cells died.

[1363] The wrong projections were cut by the Lewy body dementia.

[1364] Certainly dopamine neurons die in Lewy body dementia.

[1365] Those are neurons that give rise to much of the feelings of reward and pleasure that we experience, among other roles.

[1366] So clearly, in his case, there could have been a very concrete cellular neurological issue that was progressive and pushed him to that point.

[1367] But were you about to make a point about broader that if there is not in your logical degeneration?

[1368] Yeah, so in his case, not knowing that, it could have been simply that, let's say he had an internal psychic pain state, and he was in sort of a compensated mode.

[1369] for much of his life, able to generate enough joy from his comedy and his social interactions.

[1370] But eventually later in life, you know, those things drop away, the balance shifts.

[1371] You get tired of fighting the pain for that long.

[1372] And then, so you've got this time -dependent, non -stationarity that happens, and then the same symptom becomes no longer tolerable in the end.

[1373] What is autism?

[1374] What do we know about autism?

[1375] human beings exist on a spectrum of how social we can be and this is pretty interesting actually scientifically but also very important clinically there are hypersocial states where people are almost too social there are chromosomal deletion states where people have instant affinity and bonding and rich deep seeming connections with people very verbal on the other end people with autism spectrum disorder are not able to keep up with social interactions and it's a spectrum.

[1376] Some have mild to moderate difficulties.

[1377] They may have inability to understand what the next thing to do in a social situation is, but may have perfectly good language abilities.

[1378] And as you progress further along the spectrum, that gets more and more severe so they can't make eye contact because it's too overwhelming to think about what has to be done next if a person looks in a particular way.

[1379] And then as you go farther than language and social communication themselves break down, so there's no reciprocity, there's no shared enjoyment.

[1380] And that this gets very hard then as you get to this far end of the spectrum where there's really an absence of social cognition at all and social bonding.

[1381] so why why does this exist what is it uh it's very genetic as i mentioned it's it's one of the you know top three or four most biological in the sense of most genetically determined of the psychiatric illnesses it does have these interesting positive correlations slight positive correlations with intelligence and education and the reason for that is kind of interesting to think about is there something good about it just like or at least with at least part of the spectrum.

[1382] Is there something good about it?

[1383] Just as we were talking about for depression, as you could say for mania, as you could say for schizophrenia.

[1384] And here it's kind of interesting to think about the underlying science of what it means to be good at a social interaction.

[1385] Someone who is very good at a social interaction is incredibly good at dealing with unpredictable information, is able to handle this torrent of information coming through.

[1386] rapidly changing model of the other person and of the interaction and their model of you, your model of them, with each word that changes, with each new bit of information that comes in through the conversation, each bit of body language, all this is rapidly changing.

[1387] And some people are able to keep up with that firehose information perfectly well.

[1388] But that's a special brain state to be in.

[1389] That's working with unpredictability.

[1390] The only way that can be done is most likely by constantly running models of what the other person might be about to say.

[1391] So you can't stop and think, oh, what did that word mean?

[1392] What did that shift in eye contact mean?

[1393] You know, what did they mean together?

[1394] There has to be some advance work going on where you're predicting what's going on if you're to keep up with a rich and fast social interaction.

[1395] Now, on the flip side, there are.

[1396] brain states that maybe don't have to work so fast that are extremely important still dealing with something that's not moving or that's predictable still complex like you know mathematical proof or a very complex arrangement of geometrical shapes a large number of individual non -moving things there's a possibly a way of being that's particularly good at dealing with these static unmoving or predictable situations and less so with these rapidly changing social situations.

[1397] And so the way I conceptualize autism is, these are people whose brains are not so good with the high bit rate unpredictable information, but may be quite good at given enough time, given the grace to work with the system, to look at it from different angles, to take different perspectives with the confidence that it's not changing in between perspectives, that's a brain state that's valuable.

[1398] It's something that has probably has contributed to a lot of the success of the human family, being able to design something, being able to consider all the different contributions to a static, predictable system.

[1399] So Autos, in a sense, is a spectrum that has identified identifiable characteristics about the way people deal with dynamic information often express itself as like social dynamic information.

[1400] But you critically, your use of the word often there is really, I think, smart because it's not just social interaction that is a challenge in autism.

[1401] And so many people conceptualize it purely as a social dysfunction disorder.

[1402] But it's really any unpredictable information.

[1403] that's a problem, that's a challenge for people on the spectrum.

[1404] They react very negatively to unexpected sounds, even if not social sounds, unexpected lights, unexpected touches.

[1405] And so it's really unpredictable information that is, in my view, the core problem with the processing in autism, not just social.

[1406] Social just shows up because it's so unpredictable.

[1407] Yeah, it's so interesting.

[1408] I mean, I tried to not to think about that stuff.

[1409] I'm afraid of thinking about disorders and things like that because just like I don't like sort of economics or game theory, I wouldn't be careful with it because it, whenever you have a category or a model, it's too easy to just, for everything.

[1410] It's the OCD thing.

[1411] I like models too much.

[1412] I like categories too much.

[1413] The moment you acknowledge yourself, well, I have an eating disorder, for example, or something like that.

[1414] as opposed to just being a, well, I'll just leave it at that from my own critical understanding of myself.

[1415] Let's just say, I don't know how to moderate eating fruit.

[1416] People make fun of me, they think all fruit is healthy.

[1417] No. Well, I don't know how to moderate anything, but even fruit, that's apples and cherries, it's a nightmare.

[1418] Anyway, that's such a psychiatrist thing to say.

[1419] Very interesting.

[1420] but there's characteristics and I it's interesting to think about like for example I have trouble making eye contact but I actually as you said it now it's not that I'm shy at all in that sense it's literally I'm getting way too much information and is distracting me like I need to just close my eyes so I can like all the things that that people seem to be able to do in parallel.

[1421] It's just, you just asked me a question.

[1422] For me to think about the answer to that question, I can't have all this cool, rich, visual information coming my way.

[1423] That's literally, because I often close my eyes to think.

[1424] It's not because I'm afraid of something, whatever, it's just like too much information happening here.

[1425] Well, that's a beautiful description.

[1426] It's amazing that that is how you experience the eye contact aspect.

[1427] I think that's, I mean, you've articulated what, you know, captures it for so many people, which is that it's overwhelming.

[1428] There's just too much information just coming in through the eyes.

[1429] And to keep up with it, to know you're going to be expected to keep up with it, first of all.

[1430] So there's that aspect.

[1431] You know, you learned socially that there's going to be an expectation.

[1432] If you're making eye contact, people are going to think you're keeping up with it.

[1433] And you don't want to because you want to focus on other things and make progress in other, you know, dimensions.

[1434] Yeah.

[1435] And so then there's a strong desire to look away or to close the eyes because it's overwhelming.

[1436] It's a distraction and it's going to cause errors of understanding.

[1437] And of course, our eyes, that's part the way we use our eyes as part of the human communication.

[1438] You have to kind of be aware of that element of it.

[1439] So yeah, I mean, but it's fascinating.

[1440] You should be aware of your own self in those little characteristics, whether it's classified on some aspect of the autism spectrum or just in general, whether it's eating, whether it's depression, whether it's even like schizophrenia that would, I hope we get a chance to talk to a little bit.

[1441] Yeah, but those things are all made up of different symptoms and characteristics and use them as a super, power, I suppose, is the best we can hope for in mild cases, I guess.

[1442] I do think both brain states can't coexist at the same time.

[1443] The way of dealing with something unpredictable and dealing with something predictable, those are different ways of being, here's a huge opportunity for very creative model building in theoretical neuroscience and linking that to these data streams we're getting across the brain that we talked about earlier, these immense data sets.

[1444] of activity across the brain.

[1445] Here's where I think there could be a real convergence of theoreticians and experimentalists to say, okay, given what we know about wiring of the brain, here is what the brain state is likely to be that deals well with unpredictable information.

[1446] And here's the brain state that deals with predictable information.

[1447] Here's why they're incompatible, at the same time.

[1448] Here's why you've got to be able to detect which state you should be in.

[1449] Here's how you could switch between them.

[1450] Here's the kind of cells that you would predict, almost like predicting the Higgs boson.

[1451] Here's the kind of circuitry that I would predict should govern the switching or might make one state too sticky, too hard to get out of, right?

[1452] Yeah, yeah.

[1453] That is a huge opportunity for an interaction from the theoretical and experimental side together.

[1454] Make one state too sticky.

[1455] the sort of measure the stickiness of the state and how to lessen the stickiness.

[1456] That's right.

[1457] Get some oil in the machine.

[1458] Yes.

[1459] Yeah, what would predict the kind of oil that would work well.

[1460] What in your practice is treatment or advice for people on the autism spectrum?

[1461] So right now there's no real medical treatment.

[1462] There are behavioral treatments that are most.

[1463] effective early in life.

[1464] They make sure people don't fall too far behind.

[1465] If you're not interacting socially, you create this vicious cycle where you fall farther and farther behind because you're not interacting.

[1466] And these therapies, which are applied early in life, therapists work with the kids, train them to deal with these things that otherwise would be aversive to them, teach them how to predict things and interact.

[1467] And that has a big effect.

[1468] But it's behavioral therapy.

[1469] There's no medicine that works.

[1470] There are ways of reducing individual symptoms, though, that sometimes come along with autism, and those do respond to medications.

[1471] So you can, you know, one thing, very often my patients with autism are very anxious because they live in a world that they have a really hard time predicting what's going to happen.

[1472] And so they find, and some of these are high functioning, you know, Silicon Valley types who, you know, they may make, you know, great livings, but they're very unhappy because they're on the spectrum.

[1473] They don't understand how social interactions really work.

[1474] They're very anxiety -provoking because they don't know what to say.

[1475] They don't have any clue how anybody else knows what to say.

[1476] They're constantly worried.

[1477] They're going to say something that's completely inappropriate, and so they're very anxious.

[1478] And I can treat their anxiety.

[1479] It doesn't touch autism per se, but I can help them with their anxiety.

[1480] What I just talked about, eye contact, I am richly, even with the eyes closed and all those kinds of things, I'm richly experiencing the world.

[1481] And it's not like you're afraid of the world or you're not able, I don't know what to do.

[1482] No, I know everything.

[1483] In fact, I know way too much.

[1484] There's so many cool options.

[1485] Like at any one moment, there's all the stuff happening and it's all beautiful.

[1486] And in any one moment, you can do anything you want.

[1487] You can take off your close you can punch that guy over there you can run away you can go in for a hug you can say something profound and deep or you can say something generic or you could use so many things you can say and then and then it'll go it'll unravel in all these kinds of ways in this moment can be completely life -changing or it can be mundane and meaningless and all of those options are before you at any one moment and so it's amazing it's amazing and overwork If it's overwhelming if you allow yourself to think about it, which, whatever, exactly, like, well, unfortunately, with chess, you have a few set options.

[1488] Two -dimensional, those constraints.

[1489] There is unlimited possibilities and unlimited beautiful things happening all around you.

[1490] So I don't think there's a kind of sense that somehow you're limited in your, in the places of, in the way you can see the world and how you can interact with that world.

[1491] I am overwhelmed by the lack of limit.

[1492] That all of us should be, have you looked around?

[1493] You can do whatever the hell you want.

[1494] Nobody will remember you anyway.

[1495] All of us will be dead one day.

[1496] You could do anything.

[1497] You can, I don't know.

[1498] You can get naked and run around the city as long as you're not hurting anybody.

[1499] And it doesn't matter.

[1500] Austin, anyway.

[1501] Austin.

[1502] Yeah.

[1503] Exactly.

[1504] like a to -do item for anybody living in Austin, for sure.

[1505] But, you know, the spectrum is an interesting concept because that is, you know, when I say, when I refer to the spectrum, I'm actually referring to, it's a precise clinical term, but you're right, it's been co -opted more broadly, and it is widely used, and it can be an unfair categorization of someone who's socially and occupationally very healthy.

[1506] And that is critical because we don't define a disorder unless there's social or occupational dysfunction.

[1507] It doesn't matter what the symptoms are.

[1508] I've had patients who are pleasantly hallucinating, so frankly, psychotic, but doesn't affect their lives.

[1509] So I don't give that person as diagnosis because there's not social or occupational dysfunction.

[1510] same with anything on this you know any of the diverse symptoms of autism spectrum disorder if someone has them but they're successful socially and occupation we don't say that there's a disorder but then you're right that the concept of the spectrum does become a useful you know pigeonholing device which is maybe not not the best thing yeah and eye contact is interesting one is an interesting one.

[1511] I'm torn on it.

[1512] I'm torn about the usefulness of eye contact because people kind of make fun of it, but let me just say one thing about eye contact and about life in general.

[1513] It's okay to be weird.

[1514] But like some people, when you have your eyes closed and there's that weird, what is happening to this creature?

[1515] Like you see a weird creature on the side of the road.

[1516] It's interesting.

[1517] And you want to, I mean, the weird stuff, I'm going to go back to Robin Williams with the, that's the good stuff, right?

[1518] He has that whole speech about him and his wife and what he loves all the little peculiarities, all the weird stuff.

[1519] And that, like, let those flourish.

[1520] Let those, like, celebrate those in yourself and not in some kind of woke way, but in some, like, very human way.

[1521] This is what makes us, this is the weirdness.

[1522] Yeah.

[1523] I'm 100 % on board with that.

[1524] And I don't think, you know, people who are happy and who have people in their lives who are happy with them.

[1525] These are, I think, let the weirdness flourish.

[1526] Let the, all the different ways members of the human family can be different.

[1527] Let's see them all.

[1528] That's one of our, that's one of the joys of being alive is seeing all the ways we can be human.

[1529] And I think about it all the time.

[1530] Why do we have all these ways of being human?

[1531] And even within one individual, you go through phases of life where you express different sides of your way of being, which is also a pretty fun opportunity, right?

[1532] You can go through phases where you're in one mode and phases when you're another mode, and let that, you know, just let that flourish too.

[1533] Let the ways that you can be you vary as well.

[1534] I think that's important for people to explore.

[1535] And I should, like, because if you can address the internet, but I would like to sort of ask the internet to celebrate the, uh, the weirdness of people.

[1536] Like that's, uh, it's the Robin Williams, uh, people call these imperfections, but they're not.

[1537] That's the good stuff.

[1538] For any one individual person, find the weird stuff and celebrate it as opposed to what the internet often does, which is find the weird stuff and criticize it.

[1539] Because when you criticize the weird stuff, you're creating conformity, which is another human thing.

[1540] But that conformity creates a boring world.

[1541] You want the weird.

[1542] You want the crazy.

[1543] That's what fun is made of.

[1544] That's the foundation of humor.

[1545] and all the ways in which we deal with the suffering in the world, with the injustices in the world is like this huge variety of weird.

[1546] Yeah, I don't know.

[1547] And that's what at the depth of psychiatry is like you want to acknowledge the weird, celebrate the weird, like step around it to find the particular aspects of weird that are debilitating, like you said.

[1548] They're somehow negatively affecting your ability to function in the world as opposed to trying to shut it all down.

[1549] That's right.

[1550] Well, on that topic, I mean, I'd love to talk to you about schizophrenia.

[1551] What is schizophrenia from your research and from your general understanding and what is the full landscape of suffering and wisdom that schizophrenia explores?

[1552] Schizophrenia is a state where there is a break from reality, and so this can show up, as we call them, the positive symptoms of schizophrenia.

[1553] These include hallucinations, hearing something or seeing something that's not there, usually auditory hallucinations.

[1554] Paranoia, people can have complex fears, delusions, which we call fixed false beliefs.

[1555] people get an extremely unshakable but completely implausible idea about something.

[1556] Sometimes it relates to themselves, sometimes to the world.

[1557] These we call the positive symptoms, break from reality as we know it.

[1558] Then there are the negative symptoms that come with it, and these are progressive.

[1559] These are flattening of emotion, as we call it, so starting to express less and less, positive emotion, ending more in a neutral or flat state.

[1560] thought disorder, inability to work with complex patterns of planning or thinking, so you can't make plans, you'd have poor working memory, you can't keep track of where you were in a conversation, in a sequence of actions.

[1561] So poor and impaired working with the thoughts of oneself and then these positive symptoms of break from reality.

[1562] Okay, now why does this, why do these come together?

[1563] what's the neurobiology of it?

[1564] Again, we don't know.

[1565] Schizophrenia extremely genetically determined.

[1566] If you look at the numbers, could be upwards of 80 % genetically determined.

[1567] Wow.

[1568] 1 % of the human population around the world, it's universal, okay?

[1569] It's not confined to any one culture, not even really biased in one, you know, culture or another, about 1 % around the world.

[1570] And has this progressive, quality to it untreated.

[1571] So it's very interesting.

[1572] There's a break that happens.

[1573] We call it first break when someone experiences their first disruption of reality.

[1574] They can have a completely typical life up into that point.

[1575] So you might have a, and I've seen, you know, just heartbreaking cases of like this, like this in the Stanford emergency room where a kid who's come there has been extremely, you know, high functioning in that sense of academic achievement and athletic and interpersonal and then comes to college.

[1576] Usually in men, it's around 18, 19 when the first break happens.

[1577] Some terrifying paranoia hits or some auditory hallucinations start.

[1578] They're getting screamed at by a voice in their head.

[1579] So devastating.

[1580] With women comes on also often a little later, sometimes in the 20s.

[1581] And it can be progressive.

[1582] If it's not treated, it just progresses.

[1583] and progresses, the voices become overwhelming, the delusions and paranoia extend and expand, the negative symptoms particularly become more and more severe, so one can't even maintain thoughts in any sort of ordered fashion.

[1584] And then eventually, you know, it can be fatal, can lead to suicide, it can lead to erratic behavior that leads to accidents.

[1585] Now, it can be treated, There are medications that help, fortunately.

[1586] They have side effects, so they're not perfect.

[1587] You can have movement problems and actually a whole host of different side effects that come from the medications.

[1588] But we can help people now with schizophrenia very, very significantly.

[1589] But the amazing thing, and this is emblematic of where psychiatry stands, we don't have the deep understanding.

[1590] Just like with depression, we don't have that heart as a pump level of understanding.

[1591] understanding that we'd like to have with schizophrenia, despite it being so biological, so genetic in its nature.

[1592] So is there a way once, a way to return to the other side of the first break?

[1593] So when you have a break with reality, is there a way to kind of stitch it together?

[1594] Yeah.

[1595] So some people, that works.

[1596] But we don't really know how.

[1597] So medications, antipsychotic medications, we call them.

[1598] They block a particular neurotransmitter receptor called the serotonin 2A receptor, and they modulate dopamine as well and other neurotransmitters.

[1599] These can take someone who's actively hallucinating, actively paranoid, put them back in a completely normal state, and some people stay that way indefinitely.

[1600] So you can bring people back from that, back to the other side, have it stitched together.

[1601] More typically, you'll end up in some intermediate state where symptoms are reduced powerfully, but there might be still something there, and you've got a drop down in functioning that may be persistent for a while.

[1602] But concepts, what physically is going on.

[1603] One idea is that it's communication within the brain.

[1604] One part of the brain is not able to tell other parts of the brain what it's doing.

[1605] And so the auditory hallucinations are very interesting in this regard.

[1606] They often have this conversational inner monologue like quality.

[1607] As we're walking along the street, we may have an inner monologue thoughts about what's going on.

[1608] If we see somebody we don't like, we may have a thought out where somebody would punch that guy, something like that, or maybe I should punch that guy.

[1609] But these are so far below where we would ever, you know, act or even think of acting.

[1610] but they're just things that come up and in people with schizophrenia, those inner thoughts, that inner monologue is not recognized as the inner monologue of the self, and so it's perceived as something coming from the outside or from inside but from another entity.

[1611] Another, oh, another.

[1612] I thought you meant like another room inside the same building.

[1613] Another room inside there, yeah.

[1614] Yeah.

[1615] and so that's so it's a it could be conceptualized as a communication within the brain problem notifying what's another part of the brain what's going on and there's some evidence consistent with that i don't know if you can help with this but i sometimes so i've been talking to quite a few homeless folks recently just that's what i do is i hang out at night um and uh talk to interesting people and some of them and I've known people in the past who suffer from schizophrenia and some of them like self will describe as that as something they suffer from and they seem to understand something deeply about this world.

[1616] I don't know if it's correlated or maybe it's another aspect of depression, all those things that I've encountered in my own life is maybe just the struggle and the suffering has taken you through a life where you think deeply about life.

[1617] There's self -reflection that society forces on you because it's a disorder of some kind.

[1618] It's interesting.

[1619] I guess my only sort of anecdotal observation is people who suffer from schizophrenia seem to be very interesting and very thoughtful in a non -linear way about the world.

[1620] I've noticed that it's not always positive.

[1621] The unusual ways they view the world, but it's always interesting.

[1622] That could be conspiratorial thinking too.

[1623] But the theories they have about the way the world functions, often very well read, which is also interesting, because they're almost like looking for helpful answers somewhere.

[1624] Absolutely, they are.

[1625] And so they're, you know, they might be citing some very interesting literature and then using that, there's a stickiness in their mind to different models of the world and trying to make sense of that world.

[1626] And those models could include conspiracy theories.

[1627] They're very attuned to complexity, and they come up with unlikely explanations, which is one of the things that makes it hard for them to function in the world is how unlikely their explanations are.

[1628] But you're right, there's a depth of consideration of the complexity of the world, and a concern about it and a work, an impulse to work to understand it that is actually quite refreshing.

[1629] But, you know, the first case in the medical literature, there was a classical schizophrenia.

[1630] There was a patient named James Tilly Matthews who had this, he sketched out for his doctor the experiences he was sensing.

[1631] And he drew himself as a cowering figure on the ground controlled by a loom, a weaving device that was sending threads, long threads, projections across space from the loom to him to his arms and to his body and controlling him from afar.

[1632] And he called us the air loom, a loom in the air.

[1633] And it was such an evocative thing because, you know, this was the start of the industrial revolution or mid, and it was where, you know, really industrial strength, you know, looms and weaving devices were really kind of the emblematic of the most complex, powerful technological achievements of the time.

[1634] And so that was the explanation available to him to explain how his body was seemingly moved without his volition.

[1635] And these days, of course, people with schizophrenia will have more technology -appropriate interpretations.

[1636] They'll have delusions of satellite or alien control or beamed information.

[1637] Very, very common to have this delusion of a government agency, you know, sending electromagnetic or radio -frequency information to control their limbs.

[1638] But it's the same thing.

[1639] It's whether it's a thread from an industrial revolution loom or, you know, RF, you know, radiation.

[1640] It's the same thing just adapted to the moment explaining, trying to explain the world they live in and their relationship to the world.

[1641] But unconstrained by sort of the thing that's socially acceptable, which is both refreshing and dangerous.

[1642] Yes.

[1643] I wrote down a question.

[1644] Why do we cry?

[1645] Are tears a window into some depths that we ourselves don't know?

[1646] I almost want to make fun of myself for that question, but you do talk seriously about crying in the book.

[1647] In fact, the whole first chapter really tussles with crying as a, why do we do it?

[1648] What does it mean?

[1649] Why is it involuntary?

[1650] It seems like a weakness, right?

[1651] It's because it's so involuntary, and it's reflecting something true and inside.

[1652] At the level of the individual, that seems like a problem, right?

[1653] Wouldn't it be better if we could control it?

[1654] If we could not show that emotion when it's not useful, show it when it's useful?

[1655] but it's not.

[1656] It's largely involuntary.

[1657] And so there's a value to it, I think, as an honest reporter of a need, of hope and frailty at the same time.

[1658] I'm a human being.

[1659] There's a frailty to myself or my situation where I need social help.

[1660] I need help from my community.

[1661] I have hoped that that is possible.

[1662] but I'm not enough for myself.

[1663] I need the community.

[1664] That, I think, is what the social signal of crying is.

[1665] Now, people have studied crying.

[1666] It's an extreme, you can quantify the extent to which the presence of tears on a face triggers reactions in onlookers.

[1667] And you can show the same face in the presence or absence of tears and show that to people under quantifiable and rigorous, you know, psychological conditions.

[1668] And tears are much more powerful at stirring the desire to help in viewers than any other facial feature.

[1669] Which is pretty interesting that it's the honest one that's also the most powerful, right?

[1670] It kind of indicates there's a certain logic to our design as social beings that we have an honest report.

[1671] That's hard to control.

[1672] But is it well understood how that connects to the internal state of emotion?

[1673] Yeah.

[1674] There are long -range projections that come.

[1675] So where is crying generate?

[1676] This is the confusing thing about it.

[1677] So we have a little tear duct, the lacrimal gland, that leads to the release of fluid.

[1678] It ejects fluid, and it comes out.

[1679] And those, of course, that whole system was designed to keep the eye, clean to wash out particulate irritants.

[1680] So it's a long -standing, as long as we've had eyes and have been out of the water in our evolution, we've needed this sort of thing.

[1681] So long -standing biological structure recently co -opted, it seems, by our evolution as social primates.

[1682] Now, how could that happen?

[1683] Well, the lacrimal gland is controlled by structures in the ponds, which is a structure deep in our, just above our neck, between our neck and our head.

[1684] And reflecting its ancient origin, right?

[1685] As you go farther down toward the spinal cord, these are the more basic early evolved structures.

[1686] And in the ponds, that's where breathing is controlled, tear duct contraction.

[1687] And what we found, and with optogenetics, we helped sort this out.

[1688] There are long -range projections from fear and anxiety regions in the forebrain that project all the way to the ponds in and around those areas.

[1689] The reason those are there, we think is to regulate the respiratory rate changes, the breathing changes of fear and anxiety.

[1690] So we know when we're in a state of fear and anxiety, we need, we cope better if we have elevated heart rate, elevated respiratory rate, more blood pumping around, more oxygenated blood, we're ready to meet the threat if it happens.

[1691] All those cells are down there in the ponds too, right next to the lacrimal duct, the tear gland neurons.

[1692] And so almost certainly this fear anxiety -induced crying arose from a very slightly misdirected long -range projection that was there to regulate breathing and a little twist, just a little misdirection, a little missing of one signpost to stop here, going on a little farther, getting to the lacrimal gland neurons gave us crying.

[1693] And we just have it, that peculiar sort of structure.

[1694] neuronal structure that resulted in that that's that's what we're stuck with and that ends up being in terms of social interaction one of the more important uh authentic involuntary displays of inner state that's right and social communication oh yeah is there other stuff like that i mean do you uh yeah i mean the human face is fascinating as a display of emotion as a display of a truth and lying and all those kinds of things.

[1695] I personally, I mean, we're all, I suppose, have different sensors that are sensitive to certain aspects of the human face, but to me it seems like the eyes are really important communication or something.

[1696] You know, I've talked to a few sort of girls about, like, Botox and stuff like that, and it always bothers me when, I guess guys can do this too, but like when women speak negatively of, I guess you can call them wrinkles at the tips of an eye, but like to me, when you smile, when you wait, not wink, but like narrow the eyes to communicate, and something is communicated, and those, that stuff is really useful, the human face.

[1697] And when it's gone, something is missing.

[1698] And a lot of little stuff, it feels like it can really, it's almost involuntary, I guess, but it's harder to describe as the presence or absence of tears.

[1699] It's like something about this person you can tell they're not bullshitting you.

[1700] And so that was what made, presumably that tier recruitment so powerful is it just landed in this very high value real estate for social communication.

[1701] If it had gone to, you know, there's a lot of neurons in the ponds that control movement of large, you know, you know, elsewhere, that would have been much less effective as a social signal than something around the eye.

[1702] So it was, however that little misdirection happened, it landed in a great area for social communication.

[1703] And because it was coming from the fear and anxiety circuits that regulate that necessary, involuntary change in heart rate and respiratory rate, it also was involuntary and that became valuable as a truth signal as social beings.

[1704] So very interesting when you about the origins of the human family, the origins of social structures, and our ability and need to call for help when there's hope, but need at the same time.

[1705] What is consciousness?

[1706] So you're actually using techniques.

[1707] I mean, even putting psychiatry aside, just looking at optogenetics, you're trying to understand some of these deep aspects of the human mind.

[1708] And maybe this is a good time to return to a question you mentioned you might have an opinion on if there's such a thing as a theory of everything for the human mind because surely answering of what is consciousness is as well that's not sure but it seems like it's a fundamental part of the human experience in the human mind and solving that question will result in solving the bigger thing about the human mind the flip side could be consciousness is just the few neurons that are generating some useful thing that make us...

[1709] It's like the sense of self that you talked about in the mice.

[1710] Maybe it's a subset of those cells that are just creating a richer sense of self, and that's it.

[1711] So this is a great question.

[1712] All neuroscientists think about this and a lot of non -nourists too.

[1713] It's the reason a lot of people came to the study of the brain is to think about consciousness and not just being awake or alert, but really what's sometimes called the hard problem of consciousness, which is what is that nature of that inner subjective sense we have, not just information processing, but feeling something about the information.

[1714] What is that inner state of subjectivity physically?

[1715] What is it?

[1716] And that's called the hard problem of consciousness.

[1717] And it's not extremely well -defined.

[1718] Question.

[1719] Everybody has sort of a sense of what it means, but it's such a hard problem because you run into paradoxes quite quickly the more you think about it.

[1720] And that is exciting also because it makes us think, actually, there's some fundamental, there's a big thing that we're missing.

[1721] The brain is not just a collection of little tricks.

[1722] There is a big, big concept.

[1723] So that's your sense of the big, because like a flip side could be with optogenetics.

[1724] You can, there's an engineering question.

[1725] Can you turn consciousness on and off, like a light switch?

[1726] Okay.

[1727] So here's where exactly consciousness frames the problem extremely well, and it frames it the following way.

[1728] So I told you earlier that we can stimulate 20 or 25 cells in the visual cortex of a mouse, and we can make it behave, and we can make its brain act as if it's seeing something that isn't there.

[1729] We have that level of control now.

[1730] We can pick out 25 neurons, play in activity, in both behavior and in the brain.

[1731] It's as if it's seeing something specific.

[1732] Okay.

[1733] Now, let's do a thought experiment, you know, a Godankan experiment, and let's play this out.

[1734] Let's say we could do the same thing for every single neuron in the brain of a human being.

[1735] Let's say we had total control, and I could do something like, I could show you a rich, deep, color, red, and you could look at it, and you would be aware that it's red, but also, You might have some feelings about it.

[1736] Something would be stirred in you, some subjective sense as you looked at that rich color red.

[1737] And then I would take away the visual stimulus, and I would, in this thought experiment, I would, using some hyperoptogenetics, I would play in exactly the same pattern of activity in every cell in your brain for as long as it was needed, whatever, 15 seconds, something like that, that exactly matched what was going on when you, were feeling that inner subjective sense.

[1738] Okay, so in that thought experiment, a question for you is, would you be feeling that same inner subjective sense?

[1739] Stimulus is gone.

[1740] Every neuron's doing the same thing because I'm controlling it.

[1741] There's a philosophical question there.

[1742] If you ask me specifically, I would say yes.

[1743] Okay.

[1744] Most people would say that because it's hard to say no, right?

[1745] It's very hard to say no. If every cell in your brain is doing what it was doing, what else could be different?

[1746] Well, most normal people would say yes.

[1747] Of course, philosophers would then start saying no. They're the ones that say, I'm in sort of to parallel, and sorry if it's a bit of an interruption, but if there's a robot that's conscious in front of you, if it appears conscious, then it's conscious.

[1748] Like, to me, of course, philosophers again speak up and say, well, no, how do you know it's conscious?

[1749] Well, how do you know anything is conscious?

[1750] And sort of as normal humans, we tend to lean on the experience versus some kind of philosophical concept.

[1751] So the great thing about what you just said, the Turing test is it's very practical.

[1752] If it acts conscious, it is conscious.

[1753] but I think that's limiting.

[1754] I like the thought experiment.

[1755] I think it's actually more informative.

[1756] And so I'm halfway, halfway to the conclusion there.

[1757] But let's take it as, your answer was yes, that you would be feeling the same thing.

[1758] Okay.

[1759] Now here's where it gets fun.

[1760] Now that every cell in your brain knows what it has to do in the sense that we know it and we're providing it, your brain cells don't need to be in your head anymore at all, right?

[1761] The only reason they're next to each other, the only reason they're wired together is to affect each other, to stimulate or inhibit each other.

[1762] But we don't need that anymore because optogenetically, we're providing that activity pattern for as long as needed.

[1763] We're providing the effect of the communication.

[1764] They don't need to be connected anymore.

[1765] They don't even need to be in your head.

[1766] I could spread your neurons all over the continent, all over the galaxy and I could still provide the same stimulus pattern over 10 or 15 seconds to all those neurons and somewhere Lex Friedman would be would have to be even though no longer existing as a physical object anymore would be feeling that subjective feeling and it's inescapable because it's exactly the same as the previous situation all the neurons have to be spatially like the locality constraint there had to be spatially close to each other.

[1767] And you talk about light opto, which is funny because, you know, light is the fastest traveling thing that we know of.

[1768] Maybe let's not put them all over the universe because we might get relativistic problems then.

[1769] Let's just keep them out.

[1770] Let's keep all your neurons.

[1771] Let's spread them over North America, okay?

[1772] And let's play them out.

[1773] Same pattern of activity.

[1774] And right, it seems absurd, right?

[1775] There's no way that could be true.

[1776] There's no way that Lex would be feeling that internal sense if his neurons were spread all over North America.

[1777] And yet, it's exactly the same as the previous situation where you said, sure.

[1778] So we've got a paradox.

[1779] And this is what makes people think.

[1780] Is it a paradox, though?

[1781] Sorry, yeah, yeah.

[1782] Maybe paradox is the wrong word.

[1783] We got a problem.

[1784] We got a problem.

[1785] Because it reveals that there's something big about that internal subjective state that we're not explaining.

[1786] And we don't really have a hope of explaining in the near future.

[1787] But don't you think we would still have that?

[1788] It's just the word internal loses meaning, but don't you think we still have that internal subjective state?

[1789] If not, then where the heck is the magic coming from?

[1790] Okay, well, I just think, I think there, one of the problems that I think we need to let go of is we tend to, outside of the experience of consciousness, the hard problem of consciousness we tend to think that we individual humans are really special not the subjective experience but the entirety of it like the body that contains the thing so the local the constraint of all the stuff has to be together and it's all mine that that's a very I don't know if that has anything to do with the mechanisms that are creating this so I in fact one really nice way to break through that is to either observe or create consciousness that spans multiple organisms.

[1791] So like that say it's not an organism dependent phenomena, that the phenomena can, that's just a peculiar way it has evolved on earth.

[1792] but it's a phenomenon that doesn't have anything to do with a specific biological system.

[1793] And we have different parts of our brain exist and sometimes create complex awarenesses of things that involve different neurons that are distributed widely and that need to communicate with each other to form this joint representation, this state of consciousness.

[1794] But indeed, why do they have to be in the same head?

[1795] We don't know why that would be the case that they do.

[1796] And so that's a huge, unanswered question in the field is, is what is it that binds the activity of neurons together so they can form a joint representation.

[1797] And actually, this comes back to the dissociation experiment we talked about before, where your sense self become separated from your body, those things that were fused in a joint representation, the same concept, unitary, are now separate.

[1798] And in late 2020, we published a paper in nature showing how this could be.

[1799] We used optogenetics to drive this rhythm that ketamine and PCP cause in retrosplenial cortex, and we got different parts of the brain to be out of sync when they were active.

[1800] never able to be active at the same time, never able to form a joint representation at the same time.

[1801] And so we've got to toehold into these questions.

[1802] We don't have the answers.

[1803] And that mimics the dynamics of ketamine effects.

[1804] Exactly, exactly.

[1805] And you're able to find that kind of oscillation.

[1806] Wow, wow, wow.

[1807] So if you get even greater and greater control with more control over individual neurons and understanding, like if you think of certain neurons that having some role to play in the sense of self, you can play like an orchestra that to create certain degrees of consciousness, degrees of subjectivity, and thereby understand what is consciousness, by having a very complicated light switch, And here's the challenge is the nice thing about the thought experiment is it kind of highlights that we're going to hit a point where we're addressing some very, very fundamental questions.

[1808] What allows the activity of two sets of neurons to become mutually relevant to each other?

[1809] This is in some ways maybe one of the deepest remaining questions in neuroscience is what allows activity patterns to become relevant to each other?

[1810] Do they have to be in sync temporally?

[1811] Do they need to be, is there some other quality that we don't know about that also needs to be present to allow cells to fuse together into a joint representation?

[1812] Just so I understand, because it feels close to some very, very deep idea.

[1813] So there's a bunch of semi -distributed signals going on in the brain.

[1814] and you're saying there could be something like a theory of everything if one to exist is to understand why how and why signals close to each other start becoming relevant to each other as part of some very much bigger signal that they're producing how they coordinate us essentially, because it's very distributed.

[1815] I mean, that's a kind of, within a distributed system, how is order achieved?

[1816] And this is a very specific kind of distributed system that is one of the most intelligent that we're aware of in the known universe.

[1817] In that would maybe be something, also an understanding of the full conscious experience, too, that this kind of coordination how does the coordination between different neurons that are responsible for sense of self how do they begin to form a big picture that we see as a human experience that's really, really interesting so uniting the small and that's actually literally theory of everything uniting the small the sort of the theory of the neuron the function of the neuron with the big just the functioning of the entire model That's right.

[1818] And I think keeping a toehold in both at the cellular level of resolution and the brainwide resolution will be critical.

[1819] If you lose touch with either, I think you'll miss the big insight.

[1820] So that's what we're trying to do, keeping our, keeping grounded in the cellular resolution, trying to keep the broadest brainwide perspective and meet in the middle.

[1821] Do you think you'll see it in your lifetime?

[1822] A major breakthrough in that dimension.

[1823] I have hope.

[1824] I have hope.

[1825] It's very hard to predict what will happen with big things like this.

[1826] If we don't get there, there'll be plenty of other exciting stuff, so it's okay.

[1827] But the other aspect of this whole thing is that your life is pretty short.

[1828] That's true.

[1829] So, first of all, you can die any day.

[1830] I tend to try to think about that, that it can get in any moment because it really, really can.

[1831] And if not, it'll be soon anyway.

[1832] you think about that do you think about your mortality i do yeah it's it comes back to what we talked about earlier i you know i never think i've done enough and it's it's relevant to that for sure there's a deadline yeah do you think there's ever going to be a feeling where you sit back and you're really proud of yourself i hope so like i've done enough i've done everything there is Like, like, you know, because the thing is, a warrior has some number of battles in them.

[1833] And at a certain point, if you're deeply honest, it's like, well, this, that was a pretty good run.

[1834] As far as runs goes, that was pretty good.

[1835] And you can hang up your helmet and then go sort of drink some ale, listen to some music with the old lady.

[1836] I'd say, I did pretty good.

[1837] You think you'll get there?

[1838] you know with something with nature always has surprises for us we're always the curious mind is always after more but yeah true but biology gives us other rewards you know children and family community and one can feel good about those things biology is full of rewards but do you think about those rewards what do you think is the why of those rewards what's the meaning of life and this existence what's uh what's the why of biology what does it want from us why are all these cells very busy putting together an organism that seems to want to just be in a hurry to do stuff and uh survive but it also just doesn't it's not happening being survived like you said it's curious it keeps wanting to get into more trouble why yeah that you know that we're clearly designed for that right this is we're clearly designed to to ask why and to answer and and that i think is i don't know the meaning of all life i think a meaning of our lives is that and you know this is the the aristotelian you know happiness this is an organism is happy and animals happy if it's performing to its design right if it's it's doing what it was made for yeah well you have to understand what what's what's the design and you know, who is the designer and what were they up to and how hard is it?

[1839] Do you have to build the whole universe?

[1840] And does the design even know what the hell they're doing?

[1841] Because, you know, maybe the designer built humans to find out about themselves.

[1842] That's what I would do.

[1843] Like, if I had the power to build clones, I'll build a lot of clones and I'll get them to different trouble, to understand, like, what am I designed?

[1844] What's this body designed to do?

[1845] How far can I go that way?

[1846] Exactly.

[1847] And then, and I dissociate myself completely from having any way to know, like, that I know that person.

[1848] That's good.

[1849] I mean, I suppose you could do that in a single person's body by dissociation.

[1850] You can, you could, yeah.

[1851] But I do wonder what, if you, if you look at Earth as a collective, of humans as the collection of biological organisms, it seems that we're busy doing something.

[1852] And it just seems too beautiful and too special to be a random experiment.

[1853] It seems like it's an experiment that's cleverly designed by some forces of nature that are beyond our current understanding.

[1854] And maybe that's part of our design is to keep asking why.

[1855] He said answer.

[1856] I'm not sure that's part of the design.

[1857] The answer.

[1858] I think we're given just the sufficiently limited cognitive capability that we know how to long to find the answer and we lack the ability to find the answer.

[1859] That's basically a summary of your career.

[1860] No, I'm just kidding.

[1861] And then we give each other Nobel Prizes for having even an inkling of a good step towards the right direction.

[1862] Carl, you're an incredible human being.

[1863] I'm a huge fan of who you are as a person, who you are as a scientist, who you are as a writer.

[1864] I just thank you so much.

[1865] I'm so honored that you would sit down and talk to me today.

[1866] It was amazing.

[1867] It's been incredibly fun.

[1868] Let's do it again some time.

[1869] It's been really great.

[1870] Your insights and wit and modesty are really quite rewarding.

[1871] Thanks so much, Ben.

[1872] Thanks for listening to this conversation with Carl Diceroth.

[1873] To support this podcast, please check out our sponsors in the description.

[1874] And now, let me leave you with some words from Carl Jung.

[1875] Knowing your own darkness is the best method for dealing with the darkness of other people.

[1876] Thank you for listening and hope to see you next time.